Xiangrui Li1, Oyetunde Gbadeyan 1, and Ruchika Shaurya Prakash1
1Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Brain Imaging, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
Synopsis
In
this proof-of-concept study, we conducted an optimization assessment of
predictive models for use with real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging. Here, we utilized two existing connectome-based models of sustained
attention and mind-wandering derived in independent datasets, with each model
comprising connections predictive of positive or negative associations with
target behavior. Both models showed significant networks strengths across the
blocks of the sustained attention task, with the combined model showing
significant network strengths and capturing vigilance decrements. Our results
suggest that our two models are representative of high and low attentional
states, thus making them appropriate targets for neurofeedback.
Introduction
Real-time functional magnetic resonance
(rt-fMRI) neurofeedback is a non-invasive therapeutic approach designed to
provide feedback signal based on neural activity or connectivity to modulate
brain dynamics and enhance associated cognitive processes 1,2. Recently,
Schenoist et al. 3 provided support for the feasibility of providing
neurofeedback based on network strength of a large-scale distributed model of
sustained attention. Extending the prior literature, we are interested in
examining rt-fMRI neurofeedback to enhance attentional control in older adults.
Aging
is associated with significant cognitive decline especially on tasks of
attentional control requiring focusing on particular sources of information
while ignoring irrelevant information 4,5. However, an important step in implementing
rt-fMRI neurofeedback is to carefully select and optimize the target for providing
neurofeedback, as modulation of target neural activity or connectivity will
have important consequences for the cognitive domain of interest. As such, in
this study, we examined differences in network strengths of two
connectome-based models, derived in independent datasets, to predict sustained
attention and mind-wandering, respectively.Methods
Twenty-five
older adults (aged 65-85 years) participated in a neuroimaging session at the
Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Brain Imaging on a 3T Siemens MAGNETOM
Prisma system using a 32-channel head coil. Participants were presented with
the gradual continuous performance task (GradCPT 6) where they were shown gradually changing images of city and mountain
scenes, and were asked to respond via a button press for city, but not mountain
scenes. All
participants completed two
separate runs, each consisting of four 3-minute blocks interleaved with rest
blocks.
To examine the connectome-based
model that would be best suited for providing target signal during rt-fMRI
neurofeedback, we applied two connectome-based network masks. The first mask –
saCPM – was derived separately in a sample of 41 healthy older adults to
predict d prime, or sensitivity, which takes both correct responses (i.e.,
hits) and errors of commission (i.e., false alarms) into account 7. Similarly, using the connectome-based predictive
modeling approach, we also derived a mind-wandering connectome-based model to
predict trial-to-trial fluctuation in reaction time during a task of sustained
attention. Using data on 135 older adults from the Human Connectome in Aging
(HCP-Aging 8) project, we derived a connectome-based
model to predict reaction time variability (RT_CV= RT(SD)/RT(Mean)). As successful attentional
control is likely a byproduct of the ability to select target representations,
captured via d prime, and reduce interference associated with internal and
external distractions, assessed in here via fluctuation in reaction time, we
hypothesized that a mask derived from combining saCPM with mwCPM will likely
serve as a potent target for neurofeedback. We thus applied the saCPM mask,
including a high attention network and a low attention network, along with a
mwCPM, including a high mind-wandering network and a low mind-wandering
network, to the four blocks of the two gradCPT runs. For each of the four
blocks in every run, we computed the network strength of the saCPM masks and
the mwCPM masks and calculated the difference between the high and low
networks. The difference score essentially represented a summary statistic
indexing high connectivity in nodes associated positively with behavior and low
connectivity in nodes associated negatively with behavior. For the combined
mask, we combined high attention with low mind-wandering and low attention with
high mind-wandering.
Results
Our saCPM combined model, indexing
difference in network strengths between the high attention and low attention
networks, showed an average difference of 0.19 (p<1e-30) between the network
strengths of the two masks. This difference, as depicted in Figure 1, was seen
in all blocks of runs 1 and 2, thus suggesting that the saCPM model
differentiates between functional connections necessary for high attention from
connections associated with poor performance. Interestingly, there was no block
effect, such that across blocks there was no change in network strengths of the
two maps. In contrast, the mwCPM, as depicted in Figure 2 had a smaller effect
(~0.06); although there was a block effect such that the MW combined model
showed a lower difference in Block 1 of runs 1 and 2 compared to the latter
three blocks (p<0.022), suggesting that this model may be better suited to
detect changes in mind-wandering over time. Our combined model, shown in Figure
3, showed both a block effect (P<0.04) as well as a statistically
significant difference (average difference = 0.12; p < 1e-30) between
the network strengths of functional connections representing high attentional
control and low mind-wandering and low attentional control and high
mind-wandering.Discussion and Conclusions
Our results suggest that our two
models derived in independent datasets are representative of high and low
attentional states, thus making them appropriate targets for neurofeedback. The
increase in network strength difference of the mwCPM model with time suggests
that this model uniquely captures increasing mind-wandering in the gradCPT over
time thus adding unique variance to the model. Our future directions include providing
neurofeedback using the combined saCPM and mwCPM masks to increase the
potential for neurofeedback training to enhance attentional control in older
adultsAcknowledgements
No acknowledgement found.References
- Stoeckel, L.E., Garrison, K.A., et al, 2014. Optimizing real
time fMRI neurofeedback for therapeutic discovery and development. Neuroimage
Clin 5, 245–255.
-
Hawkinson JE,
Ross AJ, Parthasarathy S, Scott DJ, Laramee EA, Posecion LJ, Rekshan WR, Sheau
KE, Njaka ND, Bayley PJ, DeCharms RC. Quantification of adverse events
associated with functional MRI scanning and with real-time fMRI-based training. Int J Behav Med. 2012;19(3):372–381.
-
Scheinost D, Hsu T.W., et al. Connectome-based
neurofeedback: a pilot study to improve sustained attention. NeuroImage (2020),
Article 116684
-
Hasher, L, Zacks, RT. 1988. Working Memory, Comprehension,
and Aging: A Review and a New View. In: Bower, GH, editor. Psychology of
Learning and Motivation. Academic Press, p 193–225.
-
Lustig, C, Jantz, T. 2015. Questions of age differences in
interference control: When and how, not if? Brain Res. 1612: 59–69.
-
Esterman, M., Noonan, S. K., Rosenberg, M., &
DeGutis, J. (2013). In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural
fluctuations during sustained attention. Cerebral Cortex, 23(11),
2712–2723. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs261
-
Fountain-Zaragoza et al., under review
-
Bookheimer, S. Y., Salat, D. H., et al. (2019). The lifespan
human connectome project in aging: An overview. NeuroImage, 185, 335–348.