Virendra R Mishra1, Karthik Sreenivasan1, Dietmar Cordes1, Aaron Ritter1, and Charles Bernick2
1Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health, Las Vegas, NV, United States, 2University of Washington - Seattle, Seattle, WA, United States
Synopsis
Repetitive head impact (RHI) is thought to induce robust white-matter (WM)
damage which may be the risk factor for various disorders. Though single-tensor
diffusion MRI (dMRI) studies have improved our understanding of WM abnormalities
at a regional level in participants exposed to RHI, there are no studies that
attempt to understand topological WM structural connectivity changes due to RHI.
Utilizing a high spatial resolution (1.5mm3) dMRI from 12 active
male boxers and 10 demographically matched healthy controls (HC), we showed
that RHI induces a topological shift as compared to HC, and this shift is
correlated with neuropsychological scores.
Introduction
Repetitive head impact (RHI) is
thought to induce robust white-matter (WM) damage1 which may be the risk factor for
various disorders2–4. Indeed, various voxelwise single-tensor
(ST) diffusion MRI (dMRI)-derived measures such as fractional anisotropy (FA)
and mean diffusivity (MD) have shown differences in the temporo-occipital WM
tracts and forceps major5–12 due to RHI. Though such studies have
improved our understanding of WM abnormalities at a regional level in
participants exposed to RHI, there are no studies that attempt to understand
topological WM structural connectivity changes due to RHI, despite RHI being
thought of as a ‘disconnection syndrome’13. Hence, in this study, we investigated
the topological organization of WM-derived structural connectome due to RHI in
boxers and studied the correlation between various graph-theoretical measures
and exposure to fighting.Methods
Participants: Twelve
male active professional boxers (Age: 34.33±5.53 years; Age of first
professional fight: 15.17±6.44 years; Years of professional fights: 10.75±4.73;
Number of professional fights: 24.67±15.59; Education: 13.17±1.99 years) and
ten healthy male controls (HC; Age: 35.5±11.07 years; Education: 15±1.56 years)
were recruited at our center. Neuropsychological Assessment: All
participants completed neuropsychological assessments using CNS vital signs14 on a computer in a quiet room
supervised by a researcher on the same visit. Four measures, namely processing
speed, psychomotor speed, verbal memory, and reaction time were collected from
every participant. dMRI acquisition: dMRI was acquired for all
participants on a 3T Siemens Skyra using CMRR pulse sequence with 2 shells of
b=1000s/mm2 and 2500s/mm2 each with 71 diffusion encoding
directions (DEC), 8 non-diffusion weighted (b0) images interspersed between the
DEC for each shell, Multiband factor=3, GRAPPA=2, TR=5218ms, TE=100ms, resolution=1.5mm3,
and phase-encoding directions of P>>A. We also acquired an opposite
phase-encoding b0 image with the same acquisition protocol. Total acquisition
time was 18 minutes. Preprocessing: All data were corrected for eddy-current
distortion using eddy15 tools and head motion was
computed across the session for each participant. Processing: Standard
processing steps were used to fit diffusion tensors after eddy current
distortion correction in FSL. Network construction: AAL
(mainly cortical)16 and ATAG (mainly subcortical)17 atlas was used to generate 102 nodes of
the network. T1-weighted MNI152 brain was
normalized to each subject’s native diffusion space and the resultant
transformation matrix was applied to the AAL+ATAG atlas to get the nodes in the
subject’s native space. Whole-brain tractography was performed using diffusion
toolkit (http://www.trackvis.org/dtk/)18. Fibers smaller than 10mm19 or having FA<0.2 were removed from
any further analysis. Each internode connection (edge) was weighted by the product
of the number of fibers and average FA of
the fibers connecting the two nodes. Using
whole-brain WM connectivity, we also computed subject scores for each
participant20. Graph-theoretical measures: Various global and local
graph-theoretical measures were computed using GRETNA21. Various sparsity
thresholds (5-40%, step=1%) were used to identify the minimum sparsity at which
the network is fully connected. A rich-club analysis was also performed to
understand whether the structural connectivity pattern has preferentially
organized to form distinctive network hubs in both groups. Statistical analysis: Network-based
statistic (NBS)22 was used to statistically quantify
differences in the weighted structural connectivity pattern between the groups.
A linear regression (using PALM)23 between graph-theoretical properties
and neuropsychological scores was performed to further understand the
neuroanatomical correlates of the neuropsychological scores. All statistical
comparisons were corrected for family-wise error at pcorr<0.05. Of
note, age and education were utilized as covariates of no interest.Results
As expected, significantly lower
processing speed and verbal memory were obtained in boxers as compared to HC. Average
head motion along the slice encoding direction was less than 1.5mm for all
participants and was not found to be significantly different between the groups
(p=0.84). NBS revealed weak structural connectivity in boxers involving the thalamus,
caudate, hippocampus, and frontal WM tracts (Fig.1B). Subject scores derived
from whole-brain connectivity was significantly weaker in boxers (Fig.2).
Subject score in HC was positively correlated with processing speed and
reaction time. The network of both boxers and HC achieved a plateau of
whole-brain connectivity at the sparsity threshold of 7%, although the number
of nodes connected at 7% sparsity was significantly less (p=0.007) in boxers.
HC showed significantly lower small-worldness at sparsity of 5% and 18%. Local
efficiency of the whole network was significantly positively correlated with
processing speed in HC (Fig.3). No local measures were different between the
groups. Both boxers and HC showed the presence of rich-club. However, the brain
regions exhibiting rich-club properties in boxers were only bilateral
hippocampus and left middle cingulum while HC exhibited rich-club properties in
the bilateral hippocampus, bilateral superior frontal gyrus, right
supplementary motor area, bilateral insula, bilateral caudate, left putamen,
and left anterior and middle cingulum (Fig.4). This observation was accompanied
was significantly lower rich-club and feeder edge strength in boxers (Fig.4). A
significant correlation was observed between neuropsychological scores and rich-club
edge strength and feeder edge strength for both HC and boxers (Fig.5).Discussion and Conclusion
Our
study shows that RHI induces a topological shift that is correlated with neuropsychological scores. Our study also suggests
that there is a minimum threshold of whole-brain WM-derived structural connectivity as
there was an inverted U-shape pattern that was obtained for subject scores in
HC.Acknowledgements
This study is supported by the National Institutes of Health
(R01NS117547 and P20GM109025), a private grant from the Peter and Angela Dal
Pezzo funds, a private grant from Lynn and William Weidner, a private grant
from Stacie and Chuck Matthewson and the Keep Memory Alive Young Scientist
Award at Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health. The Professional
Fighters Brain Health Study is supported by Belator, UFC, the August Rapone
Family Foundation, Top Rank, and Haymon Boxing.References
1. Briggs DI, Angoa-Pérez
M, Kuhn DM. Prolonged Repetitive Head Trauma Induces a Singular Chronic
Traumatic Encephalopathy–Like Pathology in White Matter Despite Transient
Behavioral Abnormalities. Am J Pathol. 2016;186:2869–2886.
2. Bazarian JJ,
Cernak I, Noble-Haeusslein L, Potolicchio S, Temkin N. Long-term neurologic
outcomes after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. United States;
2009;24:439–451.
3. Bigler ED.
Traumatic brain injury, neuroimaging, and neurodegeneration. Front Hum
Neurosci. Frontiers Media S.A.; 2013;7:395.
4. Jordan BD. The
clinical spectrum of sport-related traumatic brain injury. Nat Rev Neurol.
England; 2013;9:222–230.
5. Hulkower MB,
Poliak DB, Rosenbaum SB, Zimmerman ME, Lipton ML. A decade of DTI in traumatic
brain injury: 10 years and 100 articles later. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. United
States; 2013;34:2064–2074.
6. Mishra VR,
Zhuang X, Sreenivasan KR, et al. Multimodal MR imaging signatures of cognitive
impairment in active professional fighters. Radiology. 2017;285.
7. Ng TSC, Lin
AP, Koerte IK, et al. Neuroimaging in repetitive brain trauma. Alzheimers Res
Ther. BioMed Central; 2014;6:10.
8. Orrison WW,
Hanson EH, Alamo T, et al. Traumatic brain injury: a review and high-field MRI
findings in 100 unarmed combatants using a literature-based checklist approach.
J Neurotrauma. 2009;26:689–701.
9. Shin W,
Mahmoud SY, Sakaie K, et al. Diffusion Measures Indicate Fight Exposure-Related
Damage to Cerebral White Matter in Boxers and Mixed Martial Arts Fighters. Am J
Neuroradiol. 2014;35:285–290.
10. Wintermark M,
Sanelli PC, Anzai Y, Tsiouris AJ, Whitlow CT. Imaging evidence and
recommendations for traumatic brain injury: advanced neuro- and neurovascular
imaging techniques. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. United States; 2015;36:E1–E11.
11. Zhang L, Heier
LA, Zimmerman RD, Jordan B, Ulug AM. Diffusion anisotropy changes in the brains
of professional boxers. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2006;27:2000–2004.
12. Zhang L, Ravdin
LD, Relkin N, et al. Increased diffusion in the brain of professional boxers: a
preclinical sign of traumatic brain injury? AJNR Am J Neuroradiol.
2003;24:52–57.
13. Guye M, Bettus
G, Bartolomei F, Cozzone PJ. Graph theoretical analysis of structural and
functional connectivity MRI in normal and pathological brain networks. MAGMA.
Germany; 2010;23:409–421.
14. Gualtieri CT,
Johnson LG. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test
battery, CNS Vital Signs. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21:623–643.
15. Andersson JLR,
Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects
and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage. United States;
2016;125:1063–1078.
16. Tzourio-Mazoyer
N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of
activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI
single-subject brain. Neuroimage. United States; 2002;15:273–289.
17. Keuken MC,
Bazin P-L, Crown L, et al. Quantifying inter-individual anatomical variability
in the subcortex using 7 T structural MRI. Neuroimage. United States;
2014;94:40–46.
18. Wang R, Wedeen
VJ. TrackVis.org. Proc Intl Soc Mag Reson Med. 2007. p. 3720.
19. Cheng H, Wang
Y, Sheng J, et al. Optimization of seed density in DTI tractography for
structural networks. J Neurosci Methods. 2011/09/29. 2012;203:264–272.
20. Mishra VR,
Sreenivasan KR, Zhuang X, et al. Understanding white matter structural
connectivity differences between cognitively impaired and nonimpaired active
professional fighters. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40:5108–5122.
21. Wang J, Wang X,
Xia M, Liao X, Evans A, He Y. GRETNA: a graph theoretical network analysis
toolbox for imaging connectomics. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:386.
22. Zalesky A,
Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Network-based statistic: identifying differences in
brain networks. Neuroimage. United States; 2010;53:1197–1207.
23. Winkler
AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE. Permutation inference for the
general linear model. Neuroimage. United States; 2014;92:381–397.