Chu-Shin Peng1, Shang-Cheng Chiu2, Fan-Chi Hsiao2, Chih-Mao Huang3, Chi-Yun Liu1, Chien-Ming Yang2, and Changwei Wesley Wu4
1Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan, 2Department of Psychology, National Chengchi University, Taipei, Taiwan, 3National Chiao Tung University, Hsinchu, Taiwan, 4Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan
Synopsis
To
unveil the time-of-day effect in the aging process, we recruited both young and
senior adults to participate fMRI experiments with the numerical Stroop task
before and after nap. Beyond the dorsal attention network, we found the
thalamus activity showed facilitating effect in the senior adults but the
opposite way in the young adults. Such finding supports the
compensation-related utilization of neural circuits hypothesis across age
groups, but the time-of-day effect was relatively minor compared to the age
effect.
Synopsis
To
unveil the time-of-day effect in the aging process, we recruited both young and
senior adults to participate fMRI experiments with the numerical Stroop task
before and after nap. Beyond the dorsal attention network, we found the
thalamus activity showed facilitating effect in the senior adults but the
opposite way in the young adults. Such finding supports the
compensation-related utilization of neural circuits hypothesis across age
groups, but the time-of-day effect was relatively minor compared to the age
effect. Introduction
Senior adults usually has difficulty to maintain attention persistently within a day.1 In other words, aging deteriorates the stability of the circadian rhythm, leading to poor sleep quality and poor cognitive functions. Recently, neuroimaging literature points out that the brain attention network in the elderly adults is more vulnerable to the circadian effect as compared to the young population,2 but the attention performances can benefit from an afternoon nap.3 However, such time-of-day effect in the aging process has yet been fully understood in the field of neuroimaging. Therefore, we evaluated the attention performances of the senior adults before and after a nap. We hypothesized a compensatory effect in the elderly brain to maintain the cognitive performance at different times of a day, as compared with the young population. Methods
We recruited 27 young (age:23.8±3.9) and 14
senior adults (age:66.4±5.2) to participate two fMRI sessions within a day: 10
am and 2pm, and they were asked to have a nap after lunch. In each fMRI session,
the participant was asked to perform the numerical stroop task (NST),4
selecting between two numbers with larger magnitude, composed of six 36-sec blocks
(2 congruent, two incongruent and two neutral). There are eighteen stimuli were
presented in each block and the blocks appeared randomly. The task-based fMRI
data and axial 3D T1-weighted were acquired at a 3T Skyra MRI at National
Chengchi University using EPI sequence: TR/TE/FA=2000 ms/30 ms/90°, 33 slices
with thickness of 3.4 mm, matrix size: 64×64, and FOV=240×240 mm2.
After standard preprocessing, a general linear model was performed to calculate
the brain activation map. Group-level analysis was performed by a 2x2
mixed-effect ANOVA (Age x Time). Multiple comparison was conducted by AFNI
3dClustSim with autocorrelation function adjustments. Results
Figure
1A shows the brain activation maps of NST, where the spatial extent of the activated
areas seemed to be larger in the morning for both age groups. Interestingly, the
thalamus activation of NST was found in the senior adults only. In Fig 1B, the reaction
time showed significance in both age and time effects (p < .001). Figure 1C
shows that the thalamus activity in the senior adults was stronger that in the
young group (p < .001). At last, the scatter plot (Fig.1D) denotes that reaction
time has slight positive correlation (r =.13) with thalamus activity in the
young group, but the results in the senior adults showed a negative correlation
(r
=-.19) .Discussion and Conclusion
Compared
to the young adults, senior adults had thalamus activation of the thalamus to
improve task performances (reduced reaction time). Such finding may be
interpreted as part of the CRUNCH model in the aging process, i.e., thalamus may
serve as a compensation function in NST, especially in the senior adults.
Nevertheless, though the two groups showed different trends of thalamus
activity at different timing, both groups did not show the timing effect at the
current stage. Further investigations with more sample size are required to
further unveil the time-of-day effect in the aging process. Acknowledgements
This research was supported by Taiwan Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST 108-2321-B-038-005-MY2) and Taiwan National Health Research Institutes (NHRI-EX108-10829E1).References
1.
Fortenbaugh
FC, DeGutis J, Germine L, et al. Sustained attention across the lifespan in a
sample of 10,000: Dissociating ability and strategy. Psychol Sci. 2015; 26(9):
1497–1510.
2.
Anderson
JAE, Campbell KL, Amer T, Grady CL, Hasher L. Timing is everything: Age
differences in the cognitive control network are modulated by time of day.
Psychol Aging. 2014;29(3):648-657.
3.
Li J,
Cacchione PZ, Hodgson N, et al. Afternoon napping and cognition in Chinese
older adults: findings from the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study
Baseline Assessment. J Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2017;65:373-380.
4.
Huang
CM, Polk TA, Goh JO, et al. Both left and right posterior parietal activations
contribute to compensatory processes in normal aging. Neuropsychologia 2012;
50: 55-66.