Javier Gonzalez-Castillo1, Daniel A Handwerker1, and Peter A Bandettini1,2
1Section on Functional Imaging Methods, NIMH, Bethesda, MD, United States, 2FMRI Facility, NIH, Bethesda, MD, United States
Synopsis
Wakefulness fluctuations during
rest are a key confound for dynamic functional connectivity. Yet, tracking such
fluctuations is not trivial when lacking concurrent EEG and/or eye-tracking.
Recent work suggests that ultra-slow CSF fluctuations accompany descent into
sleep. Here we evaluate how such fluctuations help track wakefulness in rest
scans acquired on non sleep-deprived subjects using sequences not optimized for
detecting such inflow-related fluctuations. We conclude that those fluctuations
can be easily detected in other samples, and that they may provide valuable time-resolved
information about fluctuations in wakefulness, as well as a means to segregate
subjects according to their overall wakefulness levels.
Introduction
During resting fMRI scans, subjects’
level of consciousness fluctuates, yet such fluctuations are often ignored despite
their confounding role when estimating dynamic functional
connectivity (dFC)1.
The main reason for ignoring these fluctuations is that tracking them is not
easy. It either requires additional concurrent recordings (e.g., EEG2 or eye tracking) or the
use of advanced analytical methods (e.g., classifiers trained on dFC
estimates)3.
Although simpler methods exist, they are not as robust4.
Recently, it was reported that sleep is accompanied by the appearance of ultra-slow
fluctuations in CSF inflow; and that such fluctuations can be identified in BOLD-fMRI
rest scans optimized for their detection (i.e., TR < 500ms with the forth
ventricle sitting on the bottom slices to maximize inflow weighting)2.
Here we evaluate how well those
findings generalize to a different pre-existing sample of 100 subjects with
acquisition parameters not optimized for detecting those CSF
fluctuations of interest. In particular, we study how well the amplitude of
those ultra-slow CSF fluctuations track long periods of eye closure. We do this
for two different ROIs, one located in the most inferior portion of the CSF,
and another one in the 4th ventricle.Methods
DATA: The first rest scan with concurrent eye tracking from 100 subjects in the S1200-7T Release from the HCP5.
DATA PROCESSING:
Un-preprocessed timeseries were filtered to minimize respiration signals [0.16 – 0.4Hz]. In the same step, estimates of head-motion were
removed from the otherwise un-preprocessed fMRI data.
CSF SIGNAL ANALYSIS:
First, we created two ROIs per subject (Fig. 1.A): one for the 4th
ventricle (blue), and one for the central canal (CC; orange). The CC-ROI
sits near the lower end of the imaged FOV (as the 4th ventricle did
on the original study2). Then, for each ROI, we
performed the following steps: 1) compute mean representative time-series (Fig
1.B); 2) generate a spectrogram (Fig. 1.C) using the continuous wavelet
transform [Complex Morlet waveforms]; 3) compute the mean amplitude of
fluctuations in the frequency range [0.015 – 0.06 Hz] (Fig 1.D); which
corresponds to those reported to be associated with sleep; 4) calculate the
area under the mean amplitude traces (AuSBF: Area under Sleep Band
Fluctuations). Overall, a larger area indicates a more prominent presence of hypothesized
sleep-related fluctuations in CSF for a given subject.
EYE TRACKING: HCP
provides pre-processed concurrent eye-tracking data. These include, time-series
of pupil size for the left eye (Fig 1.E), as well as information regarding the
location of all detected fixations (Fig. 1.F). Periods with a value of zero for
the eye pupil traces indicates prolonged periods of eye-closure (red circle in
Fig. 1.F).Results / Discussion
Fig.2 shows the average
temporal evolution of the amplitude of CSF fluctuations in the 0.015-0.06Hz
band for both ROIs (Fig. 2.A), pupil size (Fig. 2.B) and time of eye closure
(Fig 2.C). Those graphs confirm previously reported trends that subjects lower
their vigilance and sleep as rest scan progress6.
Fig.3.A shows histograms of
AuSBF for both ROIs across subjects. We can observe a Gaussian-like
distributions with long right tails; which may signal a subset of subjects with
lower vigilance/higher likelihood of being asleep.
Fig.3.B shows a scatter plot of AuSBF for both ROIs against each other. The
linear relationship suggests both ROIs contain similar information. Fig.3.C
shows a scatter of AuSBF in the Central Canal against total duration of eye
closure. Overall, subjects with higher AuSBF tend to have their eyes closed
longer; yet one can also observe subjects with long periods of eye closure that
lack slow fluctuations in CSF (green circle; Fig.3D-F). Those could correspond
to non-compliant subjects that did not kept eyes open yet did not fall slept.
It is also possible that ultra-slow CSF fluctuations in these subjects were weaker
or even absent. In addition, we can also observe slow CSF fluctuations in the
absence of sleep for some subjects (red circle; Fig.3G-J). Close inspection
suggests that, in some instances, power in this band may be a result of
motion spikes (dashed red lines), yet drowsiness cannot be fully discarded.
Finally, Fig.4.A shows sorted values of AuSBF for all 100
subjects. A small set of subjects had elevated AuSBF compared to the rest of
the sample (Fig. 4.B-D; see examples). Those all correspond to subjects with
long periods of eye closure, and inaccurate and variable fixations away from
the center of the screen. On the other end of the tail, subjects with minimal
AuSBF kept eyes open and fixated as requested (Fig. 4.E-G; see examples).
Overall, based on the ET data, AuSBF seems to be a valuable indicator to
separate subjects according to wakefulness levels during resting scans.Conclusions
Slow CSF fluctuations, possibly
associated with sleep, can be detected in previously acquired data not
optimized for their detection. Those fluctuations may still be present during awake rest, likely due to motion
artifacts. Slow fluctuations in CSF can help easily identify subjects with
different wakefulness levels during rest scans when no other information (EEG,
ET) is available; and may help account for its confounding effects in FC
analyses. Future work should evaluate concordance with other methods such as
those based on brain templates4
and dFC estimates3. Acknowledgements
The authors thank Laura Lewis for providing valuable information regarding her original work. This research was possible thanks
to the support of the National Institute of Mental Health Intramural Research
Program. Portions of this study used the high-performance computational
capabilities of the Biowulf Linux cluster at the National Institutes of Health,
Bethesda, MD (biowulf.nih.gov). This study is part of NIH clinical protocol
number NCT00001360 and protocol ID 93-M-0170. Resting-state data were provided
by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators:
David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH
Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience
Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington
University. References
1. Laumann, T.O., et al., On the Stability of BOLD fMRI Correlations.
Cereb Cortex, 2017. 27(10): p.
4719-4732.
2. Fultz,
N.E., et al., Coupled
electrophysiological, hemodynamic, and cerebrospinal fluid oscillations in
human sleep. Science, 2019. 366(6465):
p. 628-631.
3. Tagliazucchi,
E., et al., Automatic sleep staging using
fMRI functional connectivity data. Neuroimage, 2012. 63(1): p. 63-72.
4. Falahpour,
M., et al., Template-based prediction of
vigilance fluctuations in resting-state fMRI. Neuroimage, 2018. 174: p. 317-327.
5. Van
Essen, D.C., et al., The WU-Minn Human
Connectome Project: an overview. Neuroimage, 2013. 80: p. 62-79.
6. Tagliazucchi,
E. and H. Laufs, Decoding wakefulness
levels from typical fMRI resting-state data reveals reliable drifts between
wakefulness and sleep. Neuron, 2014. 82(3):
p. 695-708.