Bo Liu1, Xiaochun Wang1,2, Le Wang2, Qiaojun Qu1, Xiang Feng3, Wei Zhang1, Bin Wang4, and Hui Zhang1,2
1College of Medical Imaging, Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan 030001, Shanxi Province, China, 2Department of Radiology, First Clinical Medical College, Shanxi Medical University, Taiyuan 030001, Shanxi Province, China, 3MR Scientific Marketing, Diagnosis Imaging, Siemens Healthcare Ltd, Beijing 100102, China, 4College of Information and Computer, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan 030001, Shanxi Province, China
Synopsis
The effect of attention
field (AF) is not only constrained to spatial positions of attention but also
to size. However, the direct link of AF size to receptive field (RF) has not
been well established. In our study, we demonstrated remarkable modulation of
AF in the early visual cortex, with regard to size and location of population
receptive field (pRF), emphatically. Specially, the differences in pRF size
between focused and distributed attention conditions increase with
eccentricity, especially in the V3 area. Considering pRF location, eccentricity
is larger under distributed attention conditions than under focused attention
conditions at the perifovea in the V3 area.
Introduction/Purpose
Visual spatial
attention is a cognitive process, where prior information about the relevance
of spatial locations is used to improve perceptual performance1,2. Visual attention spreads over a range of the visual space,
known as the attention field (AF). AF reshapes the distribution of activities across the
visual pathway, including the early visual cortex and all areas along the dorsal and
ventral visual cortical pathways3. Importantly, the
effect of attention is not only constrained to spatial positions of attention
but also to the size4. It has been
proposed that different AF size varies the patterns of neural
gain elicited by stimuli5,6, but the direct link of AF size to
receptive field (RF) has not been well established.
In this study, we used the population
receptive field (pRF) method7, which allows the
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) response to be measured at many
visual areas across the entire spatial map, to quantitatively analyze how AF
size modulate spatial representations and whether modulation occur in the early
visual cortex. Method
Eight right-handed
neurologically healthy participants (five female; ages 23–31 years) were
recruited in this study. The number of recruited subjects was within the
typical range of other studies that employed similar approaches8-13.
All subjects were scanned at a 3T whole-body MRI scanner (MAGNETOM Skyra, Siemens
Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil centered over the
subject’s occipital pole.
Functional MRI were acquired using a
gradient-echo EPI sequence with the following parameters: TR/TE = 1500 ms/30
ms, flip angle 55°, FOV= 24×24 cm2, spatial resolution = 2.5×2.5×3
mm3, 20 slices oriented orthogonal to the Calcarine sulcus, no slice
gap, total acquisition time = 2 min 39 sec. During fMRI scan, the subjects were
required to perform one-back tasks on identical streams of stimuli (ring and
wedge stimulus) presented at either central fixation (focused attention
condition) or all-around vision (distributed attention condition) while
task-irrelevant mapping stimuli traversed the visual field (Fig.1). The two
types of tasks were presented in an interleaved order, counterbalanced across
participants and repeated four times to increase the signal-to-noise ratio of
the BOLD signal.
High-resolution anatomical MR images were acquired
using a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence with the following parameters: TR/TE = 11
ms/3.3 ms, TI = 1100 ms, 172 slices, flip angle=18°, 1 mm3
resolution, acquisition time = 6 min 3 sec. T1-weighted images were used to
reconstruct the cortical surface for each subject in mrVista software
(http://vistalab.stanford.edu/software).
For fMRI data analysis, size and location
of pRF and amplitude were estimated for each voxel using a 2D Gaussian pRF
model described by Dumoulin et al7 and implemented
with the mrVista software. Voxels with at least 10% of the variance explained
by the model fit were included in the final pRF estimates. Then, pRF polar
angle and eccentricity maps of the focused attention condition trials were used
to estimate the boundaries of the early visual cortical areas (V1-V3). These regions of interest (ROIs)
were manually drawn on an inflated representation of the cortical surface using
for all conditions for a given subject. After that, we divided the voxels into
bins by their eccentricity, with each bin covering an eccentricity range of 1°
and defining 0.5°-2.5° eccentricity as the fovea, 3.5°-5.5° eccentricity as the
perifovea and 6.5°-8.5° eccentricity as the periphery, according to previous
studies14-16. Finally, two-tailed
paired t-tests and linear function were used to determine whether the
group of differences including all subjects and the relationships between the difference in bins and each bin's
corresponding eccentricity were significant. P<0.05 were considered statistically significant.Results
We reconstructed robust
spatial representations across a range of eccentricities and for both attention
conditions (Fig. 2). Our results demonstrated a remarkable attentional
modulation of early visual cortex on size, location of pRF and amplitude. The
pRF sizes increased more strongly with eccentricity under focused attention
condition (V1: slope=0.025, CI=-0.172, R2=0.766, F=19.658, p=0.004; V2:
slope=0.030, CI=-0.208, R2=0.799, F=23.911, p=0.003; V3: slope=0.080,
CI=-0.206, R2=0.875, F=31.653, p=0.001). Furthermore, the focused attention
condition showed a smaller pRF size at foveal eccentricity in early visual area
(V1: t(7) = -2.979, p=0.021; V2: t(7) = -2.650, p=0.033; V3: t(7) = -2.536,
p=0.039) and a larger pRF size at peripheral eccentricity in V3 than
distributed attention condition (V3:
t(7) = 3.015, p= 0.020), shown as Fig. 3 and Fig. 4. In addition, AF size also
affected pRF locations in V3 area, with more eccentric at perifoveal visual
degree under distributed vs. focused attention condition (t(7)=6.853,
p<0.001), shown as Fig. 5. In comparison with distributed attention
condition, the amplitudes in early visual areas were significantly higher in
the focused attention condition (V1: t(7) = 10.089, p<0.001; V2: t(7) =
20.163, p<0.001; V3: t(7) = 9.749, p<0.001).Discussion and Conclusion
We explored how
different AF sizes with
constant mapping stimuli selectively alter the patterns of pRF characteristics in the early visual cortex. Attention was restricted to a narrower field in the focused
attention condition than in the distributed attention condition. Our results
demonstrate that early visual cortexes are modulated by AF size with regard to pRF
size, location and amplitude and provide important insights into how human
early visual field representations are affected by attention.Acknowledgements
This work was supported
by the National Natural Science Foundation (81971593, 81971592, 81771824, 81702449,
61873178); the Precision Medicine Key Innovation Team Project (YT1601).References
1 Desimone, R. Visual attention mediated by
biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 353, 1245-1255
(1998).
2 Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. Neural
mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual review of neuroscience 18,
193-222 (1995).
3 Gilbert, C. D. & Wu, L. Top-down
influences on visual processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14, 350-363
(2013).
4 Reynolds, J. H. & Heeger, D. J. The
normalization model of attention. Neuron 61, 168-185 (2009).
5 Itthipuripat, S., Garcia, J. O.,
Rungratsameetaweemana, N., Sprague, T. C. & Serences, J. T. Changing the
spatial scope of attention alters patterns of neural gain in human cortex.
Journal of Neuroscience 34, 112-123 (2014).
6 Herrmann, K., Montaser-Kouhsari, L.,
Carrasco, M. & Heeger, D. J. When size matters: attention affects
performance by contrast or response gain. Nature neuroscience 13, 1554 (2010).
7 Dumoulin, S. O. & Wandell, B. A.
Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 39,
647-660 (2008).
8 Beltrán, J., García-Vázquez, M. S.,
Benois-Pineau, J., Gutierrez-Robledo, L. M. & Dartigues, J.-F.
Computational techniques for eye movements analysis towards supporting early
diagnosis of alzheimer’s disease: a review. Computational and mathematical
methods in medicine 2018 (2018).
9 Klein, B. P., Harvey, B. M. &
Dumoulin, S. O. Attraction of position preference by spatial attention
throughout human visual cortex. Neuron 84, 227-237 (2014).
10 Sheremata, S. L. & Silver, M. A.
Hemisphere-dependent attentional modulation of human parietal visual field
representations. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 508-517 (2015).
11 Sprague, T. C. & Serences, J. T.
Attention modulates spatial priority maps in the human occipital, parietal and
frontal cortices. Nature neuroscience 16, 1879 (2013).
12 Vo, V. A., Sprague, T. C. & Serences,
J. T. Spatial Tuning Shifts Increase the Discriminability and Fidelity of
Population Codes in Visual Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 37, 3386 (2017).
13 Welbourne, L. E., Morland, A. B. &
Wade, A. R. Population receptive field (pRF) measurements of chromatic
responses in human visual cortex using fMRI. NeuroImage 167, 84-94 (2018).
14 Stephenson, C. M. E., Knapp, A. J. &
Braddick, O. J. Discrimination of spatial phase shows a qualitative difference
between foveal and peripheral processing. Vision Research 31, 1315-1326 (1991).
15 Bao, Y. & Pöppel, E. Two spatially
separated attention systems in the visual field: evidence from inhibition of
return. Cognitive Processing 8, 37 (2007).
16 Yang, T., Strasburger, H., Pöppel, E. &
Bao, Y. Attentional modulation of speed-change perception in the perifoveal and
near-peripheral visual field. PloS one 13, e0203024 (2018).