Phillip G D Ward1,2,3, Edwina R Orchard1,2,3, Stuart Oldham3, Aurina Arnatkevičiūtė3, Francesco Sforazzini1, Alex Fornito3, Gary F Egan1,2,3, and Sharna D Jamadar1,2,3
1Monash Biomedical Imaging, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia, 2Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain Function, Melbourne, Australia, 3Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Synopsis
The BOLD signal detects changes in relative
concentrations of oxy/deoxy-haemoglobin. Thus, individual blood haemoglobin
levels may influence the BOLD signal-to-noise ratio in a manner independent of
neural activity. In this study, we emulate group-differences in haemoglobin by
performing a median split on 524 healthy elderly individuals based on
individual measurements of haemoglobin. When compared, the two haemoglobin
subgroups showed no differences in cognitive measures, however, significant
differences in linear relationships between cognitive performance and
functional connectivity were observed in four cognitive tests. Our findings
confirm that haemoglobin levels are an important confounding variable in
BOLD-fMRI-based studies in the elderly.
Introduction
Haemoglobin concentration is an
important mediator of BOLD signal variability in brain1 and cardiac2 MRI. It has
also been shown to vary with many factors, including sex,3 age,4 race,5 hydration
level,2 stress levels,6–8 body temperature,9 sleep
apnoea,10 cardiovascular
health,11 and hormone
levels.12 Therefore,
haemoglobin variation has the potential to confound BOLD fMRI studies where
these factors, or others, cause group differences in haemoglobin levels.
In this work, we explore this effect by examining average fMRI connectivity between
groups defined by different haemoglobin levels. We examine the
anatomical pattern of the connectivity differences for evidence as to its origin.
Finally, we compare linear relationships of connectivity and cognitive
performance between the two groups to explore the potential for haemoglobin to confound
studies of fMRI and cognition. Methods
The population was recruited for the ASPREE-NEURO study,13 a
sub-study of a broader clinical trial of aspirin in neurotypical adults
aged 70 years and over.14 We performed
a median split on 524 healthy elderly individuals (age=73.8±3.5, 275 males)
based on blood measurements of haemoglobin to produce two subgroups for each
sex. Fasting blood was
collected sent to a pathology laboratory for testing. Individuals were screened
from study entry if their haemoglobin was below 11g/dL for females or 12g/dL
for males.
Cognitive
performance was measuring using five cognitive tests: (i)
single-letter controlled oral word association test (COWAT),15 (ii)
colour trails test,16 (iii)
predicted score derived from the modified mini-mental state examination (3MS),
(iv) symbol digit modalities test (SDMT),17 and (v) the
Victoria Stroop test.18 No
differences in cognitive measures were observed between haemoglobin subgroups (all
p>0.3, t-test).
Data was
acquired prior to the administration of study medication using a 3T Skyra MRI (Siemens,
Erlangen, Germany). The fMRI protocol was a 5.16-minute eyes-open resting-state
multi-band EPI sequence (multiband factor=3, TE=21ms, TR=754ms, voxel=3.0mm isotropic,
matrix 64×64, slices=42). A T1-Weighted MPRAGE scan was also acquired (TE=2.13ms,
TR=2300ms, TI=900ms, voxel=1.0mm isotropic, matrix=240x256x192, flip angle=9o).
fMRI pre-processing included geometry distortion correction
(FUGUE), brain extraction (BET),19 intra-scan
movement correction (3dvolreg), low-pass filtering (0.01 Hz), and detrending (3dTproject,
AFNI).20 Filtered images
were then cleaned using MELODIC21 and FSL-FIX,22 with manual
training on 25 random subjects. Temporal trends from noise-labelled ICA
components were removed. The cleaned fMRI images were normalised to the 2mm MNI
template via the T1 image using ANTs.23 Finally,
the normalized and cleaned file was smoothed with a 5mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. The brain was parcellated into 82
regions (Desikan-Killiany atlas).24,25 Per-subject correlation matrices were
calculated using temporal correlations (Pearson) between each pair of regions. The
effect size of the haemoglobin median split was calculated using Cohen’s d at
each point in the matrix.
A linear correlation was estimated for each subgroup
between haemoglobin concentration and functional connectivity for each pair of
regions. A threshold of the 90th percentile was applied to binarize
the matrix, and the number of connections from each regions was calculated as a
measure of degree. This was overlaid on a vein atlas26 to compare
the anatomical origin of the strongest haemoglobin effects with the vasculature.
Finally, a
linear correlation was estimated between cognitive performance and functional
connectivity for each pair of regions. The two subgroups were compared to the whole
population to simulate the effect of haemoglobin group differences in
brain-behaviour studies.
All
analysis were performed separately for men and women as they have known
haemoglobin differences.3 Haemoglobin,
functional connectivity, and cognitive test scores were all normalized
(z-scored) prior to fitting linear models or calculating statistics.Results
Functional connectivity was significantly different
between the low-haemoglobin and high-haemoglobin subgroups for both males and females
(t-test p<10-10). The size of this
effect at a global level was small (Cohen’s d males=0.17 females=0.03). The
effect in males was consistent and broad, whereas the effect in females was anatomically
varied (Figure 1).
Linear correlations between haemoglobin and
functional connectivity were highest in sub-cortical regions and biased towards
the left-hemisphere (Figure 2). They were consistent for both males and females
and showed minimal spatial overlap with venous vasculature.
Linear coefficients between functional
connectivity and cognitive performance were consistently lower in the low-haemoglobin
subgroup compared to the high (Figure 3). The effect was present in both males
and females. Connections that showed no significant correlation between
cognitive performance and functional connectivity (where the black line
approached the zero line) still demonstrated a haemoglobin-related bias.Discussion
The results of this study demonstrate that the confounding effect
of variability in haemoglobin values is widespread across brain regions, differs
substantially between the sexes, and strongly influences functional
connectivity-cognition relationships. These results demonstrate that BOLD functional
connectivity analyses may be confounded by haemoglobin differences, especially in
studies aiming to compare groups of individuals, compare between sexes, or
examine connectivity-cognition relationships.Conclusion
Our results provide evidence that individual differences in
haemoglobin are an important confounding variable in functional connectivity
and connectivity-cognition analyses. These
findings highlight the value of performing a physiological assessment in fMRI
studies. Future functional connectivity studies should control for haemoglobin
as a confounding variable, especially in studies aiming to compare groups of
individuals, compare sexes, or examine connectivity-cognition relationships.Acknowledgements
We thank the ASPREE group for data
access. ASPREE was supported by the National Institutes of Health (grant number
U01AG029824); the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia
(grant numbers 334047, 1127060, 1086188); Monash University (Australia) and the
Victorian Cancer Agency (Australia). The Principal ASPREE study is registered
with the International Standardized Randomized Controlled Trials Register,
ASPirin in Reducing Events in the Elderly, Number: ISRCTN83772183 and
clinicaltrials.gov number NCT01038583. ASPREE-Neuro trial is registered with
Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry ACTRN12613001313729.References
1.
Xu F, Li W, Liu P, et al.
Accounting for the role of hematocrit in between-subject variations of
MRI-derived baseline cerebral hemodynamic parameters and functional BOLD
responses. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(1):344-353. doi:10.1002/hbm.23846
2. Guensch
DP, Nadeshalingam G, Fischer K, Stalder AF, Friedrich MG. The impact of
hematocrit on oxygenation-sensitive cardiovascular magnetic resonance. J
Cardiovasc Magn Reson. 2016;18(1):42. doi:10.1186/s12968-016-0262-1
3. Rushton
DH, Barth JH. What is the evidence for gender differences in ferritin and
haemoglobin? Crit Rev Oncol Hematol. 2010;73(1):1-9.
doi:10.1016/j.critrevonc.2009.03.010
4. Zauber
NP, Zauber AG. Hematologic Data of Healthy Very Old People. JAMA.
1987;257(16):2181-2184. doi:10.1001/jama.1987.03390160067028
5. Dutton
DB. Hematocrit Levels and Race: An Argument Against the Adoption of Separate
Standards in Screening for Anemia. J Natl Med Assoc.
1979;71(10):945-954.
6. Jern
C, Wadenvik H, Mark H, Hallgren J, Jern S. Haematological changes during acute
mental stress. Br J Haematol. 1989;71(1):153-156.
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2141.1989.tb06290.x
7. Muldoon
MF, Herbert TB, Patterson SM, Kameneva M, Raible R, Manuck SB. Effects of Acute
Psychological Stress on Serum Lipid Levels, Hemoconcentration, and Blood
Viscosity. Arch Intern Med. 1995;155(6):615-620.
doi:10.1001/archinte.1995.00430060077009
8. Patterson
SM, Matthews KA, Allen MT, Owens JF. Stress-induced hemoconcentration of blood
cells and lipids in healthy women during acute psychological stress. Health
Psychol. 1995;14(4):319-324. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.14.4.319
9. Thirup
P. Haematocrit. Sports Med. 2003;33(3):231-243.
doi:10.2165/00007256-200333030-00005
10. Choi
JB, Loredo JS, Norman D, et al. Does obstructive sleep apnea increase hematocrit?
Sleep Breath. 2006;10(3):155-160. doi:10.1007/s11325-006-0064-z
11. Jin
Y-Z, Zheng D-H, Duan Z-Y, et al. Relationship Between Hematocrit Level and
Cardiovascular Risk Factors in a Community-Based Population. J Clin Lab Anal.
2015;29(4):289-293. doi:10.1002/jcla.21767
12. Drinka
P. Hematocrit Elevation Associated With Testosterone Administration. J Am
Med Dir Assoc. 2013;14(11):848. doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2013.08.006
13. Ward
SA, Raniga P, Ferris NJ, et al. ASPREE-NEURO study protocol: A randomized
controlled trial to determine the effect of low-dose aspirin on cerebral
microbleeds, white matter hyperintensities, cognition, and stroke in the
healthy elderly. Int J Stroke. 2017;12(1):108-113.
doi:10.1177/1747493016669848
14. McNeil
JJ, Woods RL, Nelson MR, et al. Baseline Characteristics of Participants in the
ASPREE (ASPirin in Reducing Events in the Elderly) Study. J Gerontol Ser A.
2017;72(11):1586-1593. doi:10.1093/gerona/glw342
15. Ruff
RM, Light RH, Parker SB, Levin HS. Benton controlled oral word association
test: Reliability and updated norms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol.
1996;11(4):329-338. doi:10.1093/arclin/11.4.329
16. D’Elia
L. Color Trails Test Professional Manual. Lutz, Florida. USA: PAR
Psychological Assessmant Resources; 1996.
17. Smith
A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
Services; 1982.
18. Troyer
AK, Leach L, Strauss E. Aging and Response Inhibition: Normative Data for the
Victoria Stroop Test. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2006;13(1):20-35.
doi:10.1080/138255890968187
19. Smith
SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp.
2002;17(3):143-155. doi:10.1002/hbm.10062
20. Cox RW.
AFNI: Software for Analysis and Visualization of Functional Magnetic Resonance
Neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res. 1996;29(3):162-173.
doi:10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014
21. Beckmann
CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state
connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol
Sci. 2005;360(1457):1001-1013. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1634
22. Salimi-Khorshidi
G, Douaud G, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Griffanti L, Smith SM. Automatic
denoising of functional MRI data: Combining independent component analysis and
hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage. 2014;90:449-468.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046
23. Avants
BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, Gee JC. Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration
with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and
neurodegenerative brain. Med Image Anal. 2008;12(1):26-41. doi:10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004
24. Desikan
RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing
the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage.
2006;31(3):968-980. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021
25. Fischl
B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. Whole Brain Segmentation: Automated Labeling of
Neuroanatomical Structures in the Human Brain. Neuron.
2002;33(3):341-355. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X
26. Ward
PGD, Ferris NJ, Raniga P, et al. Combining images and anatomical knowledge to
improve automated vein segmentation in MRI. NeuroImage.
2018;165:294-305. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.049