Lanxin Ji1,2,3, Godfrey D Pearlson1,2, Keith A Hawkins1,2, David C Steffens4, and Lihong Wang4
1Departments of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States, 2Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Hartford Hospital/Institute of Living, Hartford, CT, United States, 3Center for Biomedical Imaging Research, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Tsinghua University, Beijing, China, 4Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, CT, United States
Synopsis
Neuroimaging studies show reorganization of neural resources
in older adults may compensate for cognitive decline. To effectively evaluate
neural compensation, we proposed a data-driven independent component analysis
method, and tested the measure through a longitudinal study. Twenty-six healthy older adults participated
in a 6-week physical exercise program. Gait speed, cognitive function, and fMRI
during a challenging memory task were measured before and after the program.
Results showed a positive correlation between the compensatory ability measure
and gait speed at baseline. Physical exercise improved gait speed, cognition,
and compensatory ability through increased involvement of motor-related
networks in conducting the cognitive task.
Introduction
Neuroimaging studies suggest that older adults may compensate
for declines in brain function and cognition through reorganization of neural
resources. In prior studies, analyses of neural compensation measures
have shown limitations in clinical interpretation, generalizability and lack of
control for key factors that may influence compensation, such as physical
exercise. In this study, we hypothesized that after a six-week physical exercise
program, participants would show increased neural compensation ability and
improved cognition. We used a challenging cognitive task, so that either
stronger neural response or more neurons would be activated in task-related
core neural networks, or that other neural networks that are typically not
task-related would be activated as compensatory. In this model, the number of
activated neural networks under a high cognitive-demand state should imply
neural compensatory capacity.
Methods
Twenty-six physically and cognitively healthy older adults
(mean age (SD) =74.2 (5.7) years) were recruited in this study. Seventeen of
them completed a 6-week computer-guided exercise dance program. Gait speed (a
measure of physical activity level), cognitive function, and functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) using a very challenging memory task (mean
accuracy at 33%) (Fig.1) were measured before and after the exercise program.
Gait speed was assessed using the six-minute walking test (6MWT). Cognitive
function was evaluated using a neuropsychology battery including Rey Auditory
Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Logical memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory test
(LMT), WAIS-III Digit-Symbol Substitution Modality Test (DSST), WAIS-III Digit
span, Trail Making Test (Trails A and Trails B), Stroop Color and Word Test,
and Benton visual retention test (BVRT). We used different versions of
cognitive tests before and after the 6-week exercise to avoid learning confounds.
FMRI data were preprocessed using CONN Toolbox 1 with
the standard preprocessing pipeline including slice timing, realignment,
registration to structural images, normalization to standard space, and smoothing
(6mm kernel). Spatial independent component analysis (ICA) on the memory
task-related fMRI data using GIFT toolbox
(http://mialab.mrn.org/software/gift/) identified 15 functional network
components rather than noise. We then extracted the time course within each
component for each subject. Components with a time course significantly
correlated (p<0.001) with task design were considered as “activated” by the
task, and these were counted as the number of activated networks by the task for
each subject. Visual, attentional, and left executive networks were identified as
core networks (commonly found in 77.3-97.3% of subjects). Other networks activated
by the task were regarded as compensatory. Therefore, we defined the number of
activated networks controlled for the volume of core networks as a measure of
neural compensatory ability. We examined the relationship between compensatory
ability and gait speed at baseline. For each network, the ratio of occurrence
of “activated networks” divided by number of all scans was defined as
“activated ratio.” Activated ratio changes
for each network after exercise were used to examine the network reallocation effect.
We applied paired t test to compare changes in cognitive function and gait
speed. Significant levels for the t-tests were set as p<0.05 before multiple
comparison correction.Results
At baseline, the number of activated networks significantly
correlated with 6MWT (r=0.66, p=0.015) (Fig.2), and marginally correlated to Digit
Span test after multiple comparison correction (r=0.528, p=0.035) (Fig.3a). Only
subjects whose accuracy rate was above chance (>25%) were included here.
Testing after the exercise program showed significantly
improved memory performance in LMT (p=0.001) and RAVLT (p=0.005) and increased
gait speed in 6MWT (p=0.03). Among all identified function networks, only the
motor and cerebellum networks (Fig.3A) showed increased activated ratio by 30%
and 18% respectively (Fig.3B), suggesting a higher involvement of the motor
network during memory task performance.
When we identified subjects who activated the motor network only following
exercise (n=7), we found that all of these subjects showed an increase in LMT (Fig.3C)
except for one who showed no change.Discussion
In this study, we proposed a new data-driven measure for
neural compensation ability using a highly demanding cognitive task and a brain
network-level-based method. To our knowledge, this represents the first report
of a direct impact of physical exercise on neural compensation in older adults.
A robust neural compensatory mechanism
has been considered as part of cognitive reserve. Results indicate that physical
exercise could increase the involvement of motor networks as a compensatory
mechanism under cognitively challenging task.
Interestingly, subjects who gained ability to activate motor system
“compensatorily” post- exercise also showed improved logical memory function.
We concluded that physical exercise may benefit cognition and motor function,
as well as cognitive reserve in older adults.Acknowledgements
No acknowledgement found.References
1. Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. and A. Nieto-Castanon (2012).
"Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated
brain networks." Brain connectivity 2(3): 125-141.