Previous studies have demonstrated brain activation patterns during musical playing. However, while musical playing with multiple sources of auditory inputs is essential for musical practices and group performance, it is less understood. By using an MR-compatible piano keyboard, the present study compared musical playing with or without acoustic feedback or external timing reference. Functional MRI contrasts showed BOLD signal increase in bilateral Superior Temporal Gyrus and BA 42 during musical playing with an external timing reference, which indicates a neuronal processing pattern relating to the coordination of multiple auditory inputs.
Playing music requires motor control with high temporal precision1, which is based on fast feedforward and feedback interactions between the motor and the auditory areas2,3. Specifically, feedforward processes are related to motor planning and execution to generate expected acoustic outcomes4,5, while feedback processes are related to precise motor controlling based on the discrepancy between the expected and the actual auditory outcomes6.
In group performances, an external timing reference, e.g. metronome, is essential for each member of a band to synchronize with the other players. Based on the audio-motor interaction model shown in Figure 1, playing the same melody with different acoustic inputs may involve identical feedforward but different feedback mechanisms. The feedback mechanisms involved under different auditory conditions are shown in Figure 2. When an external timing reference is available, the discrepancies between the external reference and expected acoustic outcome are explicitly fed back to the player to improve motor precision and the discrepancy between the external reference and the actual acoustic outcome is also important for feedback control. The present study aims to demonstrate the brain regions involved in the feedback control with external timing reference.
1. Zatorre, R.J., J.L. Chen, and V.B. Penhune, When the brain plays music: auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat Rev Neurosci, 2007. 8(7): p. 547-58.
2. Baumann, S., et al., A network for audio-motor coordination in skilled pianists and non-musicians. Brain Res, 2007. 1161: p. 65-78.
3. Furuya, S. and J.F. Soechting, Role of auditory feedback in the control of successive keystrokes during piano playing. Exp Brain Res, 2010. 204(2): p. 223-37.
4. Pearce, M.T. and G.A. Wiggins, Auditory expectation: the information dynamics of music perception and cognition. Top Cogn Sci, 2012. 4(4): p. 625-52.
5. Sammler, D., et al., Syntax in a pianist's hand: ERP signatures of "embodied" syntax processing in music. Cortex, 2013. 49(5): p. 1325-39.
6. Jancke, L., The dynamic audio-motor system in pianists. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 2012. 1252: p. 246-52.
7. Meister, I.G., et al., Playing piano in the mind--an fMRI study on music imagery and performance in pianists. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 2004. 19(3): p. 219-28.
8. Baumann, S., et al., A network for sensory-motor integration: what happens in the auditory cortex during piano playing without acoustic feedback? Ann N Y Acad Sci, 2005. 1060: p. 186-8.
9. Bangert, M., et al., Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction. Neuroimage, 2006. 30(3): p. 917-926.
(A) M vs None: Bilateral STG in the temporal lobe and BA 40 in the left parietal lobe. MP vs P: Bilateral STG in the temporal lobe and bilateral BA 42 in the parietal lobe. The paired t-test threshold was taken as a cluster size = 5 and uncorrected p < 0.005.
(B) The volume size of STG in both contrasts with peak coordinates and t-test value.