Letizia Casiraghi1,2, Adnan AS Alahmadi 3,4, Karl J Friston 5, Claudia AM Gandini Wheeler-Kingshott6,7,8, and Egidio D'Angelo 1,2
1Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy, 2Brain Connectivity Center, C. Mondino National Neurological Institute, Pavia, Italy, 3Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Faculty of Applied Medical Science, KAU, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia, 4UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square MS center, University College London, London, United Kingdom, 5UCL Institute of Neurology, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, United Kingdom, 6Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Italy, 7UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square MS Centre, University College London, London, United Kingdom, 8Brain MRI 3T Mondino Research Center, C. Mondino National Neurological Institute, Pavia, Italy
Synopsis
In this pilot study, we investigated whether the non-linear BOLD response to varying grip force (GF) is reproduced when observing others performing the
task. We used an fMRI squeezeball paradigm with two conditions:
action execution (AE - subjects performed the task) and action observation (AO - subjects watched a video of the task). In both
conditions, activations were detected in areas constituting the action execution-observation network
and both areas evidenced linear and non-linear relations with the GF. These
results indicate that action observation calls on the same context sensitive
and high level processing necessary for execution.
Introduction
Activation of the action execution-observation network
(AEON) in subjects observing complex motor tasks is thought to
provide a fundamental cue for imitation learning and inferring other’s intentions,
providing the basis for motor training in physiological and rehabilitative
conditions [1, 2]. Crucially, several motor areas
activated during the execution of a motor task are activated during observation
of the same task when performed by others (i.e. in the mirror system) [3, 4].
The relationship between an applied grip
force (GF) and BOLD signals shows characteristic non-linearity in
sensory, pre-motor, associative and visual areas and in cerebellum [5]. Using a
squeezeball paradigm, we asked whether the non-linear behaviour of the BOLD response to varying GF levels is reproduced when observing others performing the
task.Methods
Subjects:
8 right-handed healthy volunteers
 (5 females; aged 28
years (±5.42)). MRI protocol: 3T MRI (Philips Achieva) with a 32-channel
head coil. Sequence parameters: 1) volumetric 3DT1; 2) GE-EPI fMRI: volumes
=200, TR =2.5s, TE =35ms, voxel size =3x3x2.7mm3, FOV =192x192mm2,
slices =46, flip angle =90°, total scan-time 500s. Paradigm: Event-related fMRI squeezeball task using the whole right hand [5]; duration 8.33min; 75 trials (15*5 GFs between 20% and 60% of
maximum contraction; counter-balanced order). The task (right hand)
performed by a female was also recorded; the video included the whole hand and
forearm, on a plane background colour, palm up. Before and after the MRI session subjects
underwent a behavioural session, when they watched the video and scored the perceived
GF. In the MRI scanner, two randomised conditions were delivered: 1) [AE]
subjects had to perform the squeezeball
task themselves, while their responses were also recorded; 2) [AO] subjects
watched the recorded video. Behavioural
data analysis: orrelation between the real behavioural performance
(recorded on video) and the subject’s perception of GF was estimated. fMRI data analysis (SPM12). fMRI
data pre-processing: slice timing; realignment of volumes and re-slicing of the
mean image; co-registration of the re-sliced mean image to 3DT1; normalization
and smoothing of EPI volumes (8mm Gaussian kernel). A general linear model including polynomial expansions up to the
4th order was applied. The contrast images of (each of the five)
polynomial coefficients were entered into a between subject analysis and tested
with one sample t-tests. For the AO analysis, a similar pipeline was followed.
Here, the GF levels corresponded to those observed recorded. Significant results were
defined using a threshold of p<0.001 (minimum extent=10 voxels).Results
Behavioral
results: Data showed a positive correlation
between the applied and perceived GF (Fig.1).
fMRI results: In the AE condition, activations were detected
as follows: 0th order in
motor, premotor and sensory cerebral and cerebellar areas (p<0.05; FWE
corr); 1st order in the
motor cortex (p<0.001; k=10); 2nd
order in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes (p<0.001; k=10); 4th order in sensorimotor
and cerebellar areas (p<0.001; k=10);
-3rd order in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes as well as
cerebellum (p<0.05; FWE corr) (Fig.2). In the AO condition, activations (p<0.001,k=10) in the 0th order were seen in primary
sensory and secondary visual cortices, premotor and
parietal cortices; -1st
order in the premotor
cortex; 2nd
order in the prefrontal cortex, parietal and occipital lobes and in the cerebellum (Fig.3).Discussion and conclusion
Behavioral
results on AO endorse the recognition of the observed GF trend. Results on AE
are consistent with previously reported data [5]. AO responses were found in
regions considered to be part of the AEON [3], activated
irrespective of observed GF or in a negative linear relationship with GF. These areas are involved in movement
preparation and visual tracking of motion. Moreover, cerebellar activity indicates
a 2nd order response to the observed GF. The cerebellum is indeed involved
in the early stages of motor
learning [6] and neuroimaging
studies demonstrate that observation of movements, imagination and motion share
common substrates including the cerebellum [7,8]. These
encouraging (preliminary) results, indicate that the non-linearity is likely to
be due to high level processing of motor functions as it is present to a
certain extent in AO as well as AE. This study contributes to understanding the
functional anatomy of AO with respect to AE. The next step will be to increase
the number of subjects and merge AE and AO results (via interaction and
conjunction analysis) to create a parametric model of the AEON.Acknowledgements
Brain
Connectivity Center (BCC) - C.
Mondino National Neurological Institute,
University of Pavia; NIHR BRC UCLH/UCL High Impact Initiative; UK MS
Society; Functional Imaging Laboratory (FIL, University College of London, UCL);
Sasha Ondobaka and Francesco Rigoli for useful discussions.References
[1] Porro, C.A., et al., 2007. Enhancement of force after action observation:
behavioural and neurophysiological studies. Neuropsychologia. 45, 3114-21.
[2] Caligiore, D., et al., 2013. The contribution of brain sub-cortical loops
in the expression and acquisition of action understanding abilities. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev. 37, 2504-15.
[3] Gatti, R., et al., 2016. The effect of action observation/execution on
mirror neuron system recruitment: an fMRI study in healthy individuals. Brain
Imaging Behav.
[4] Kilner, J.M., Friston, K.J., Frith, C.D., 2007. Predictive coding: an
account of the mirror neuron system. Cogn Process. 8, 159-66.
[5] Alahmadi, A.A., et al., 2016. Complex motor task associated
with non-linear BOLD responses in cerebro-cortical areas and cerebellum. Brain
Struct Funct. 221, 2443-58.
[6] Weaver, J., 2015. Motor Learning Unfolds over Different Timescales in
Distinct Neural Systems. PLoS Biol. 13,
e1002313.
[7] Decety, J., et al., 1997. Brain activity during observation of actions.
Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain. 120 ( Pt 10), 1763-77.
[8] Grèzes, J., Decety, J., 2001. Functional anatomy of execution, mental
simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Hum
Brain Mapp. 12, 1-19.