Tommaso Gili1,2, Valentina Ciullo2,3, Daniela Vecchio2,3, Gianfranco Spalletta2,4, and Federica Piras2
1Enrico Fermi Center, Rome, Italy, 2Neuropsychiatry Laboratory, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy, 3Psychology Department, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy, 4Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, United States
Synopsis
Sense of agency (SoA) refers to the experience
of controlling one’s own actions. Temporal distortions between the action and
the effect mislead agency attribution. We investigated the
covariance between the amount of functional interactions among brain regions at
rest and SoA. We found that the functional network involved in self-agency attribution included the
premotor and somatosensory cortices bilaterally, and the right superior parietal lobule. This
provides the first evidence that functional connectivity at rest in healthy
subjects varies along with experienced SoA, implying that self-agency is
processed within an intrinsic brain functional module.
Purpose
To
look at the relationship between brain network topology and the performance in
a Judgment of Agency task in healthy subjects.Methods
Sixteen naïve healthy subjects (age(mean±sd)=(38±16);
education(mean±sd)=(15±4); males/females=6/10) participated in this study. The
behavioural tasks were administered off-resonance after the MRI acquisition. Behavioural Task Participants were
required to fixate a white cross at the centre of a black screen and to press
with their left hand the space bar once the fixation cross had disappeared. The
key press action triggered the appearance of a blue ball at the centre of the
screen. Subjects were told that the ball appearance would be either caused by
their own action or controlled by the computer. The delay between the key press
and the ball appearance was systematically varied using two different staircase
procedures: a descending one (70 trails with a starting delay of 1620 ms) and
an ascending one (70 trails with a starting delay of 90 ms). In both procedures
the delay was increased by 90 ms when subjects reported they caused the effect,
and decreased by 180 ms otherwise. On each trial, participants were required to
judge who was the agent that caused the ball appearance. MR data acquisition MRI data were collected using gradient-echo
echo-planar imaging at 3T (Philips Achieva) using a (T2*)-weighted imaging
sequence sensitive to BOLD (TR/TE=3000/30 ms, voxel size=2x2x3mm, flip angle=90°,
50 slices, 240 vol). A high-resolution T1-weighted whole-brain structural scan
was also acquired (1mm isotropic). Subjects were instructed to lay in the
scanner at rest with eyes open. Cardiac and respiratory cycles were recorded
using the scanner’s built-in photoplethysmograph and a pneumatic chest belt,
respectively. FMRI preprocessing Physiological
noise correction consisted of removal of time-locked cardiac and respiratory artefacts
(two cardiac harmonics and two respiratory harmonics plus four interaction
terms)1,2. Correction for head motion and slice-timing were
performed using FSL. Head motion parameters were used to derive the frame-wise
displacement (FD): time points with FD > 0.2 mm were replaced through a
least-squares spectral decomposition3. Data were then demeaned, detrended and
band-pass filtered (0.01-0.1 Hz), using Matlab (The Mathworks). For group
analysis maps were transformed first linearly from functional space to structural
space and then non-linearly to MNI standard space using Advanced Normalization
Tools (ANTs). Finally data were spatially smoothed (5x5x5 mm FWHM). Behavioural Data analysis For each
participant, the normalized number of trials across the sampled delays was
fitted to a Gaussian function. The delay corresponding to the curve peak value
was taken as the point of subjective equality (tPSE). Network analysis For each subject, the
square value of the BOLD correlation matrix was calculated, a threshold was
applied to ensure that the Erdos–Renyi entropy S was equal two and the
eigenvector centrality (EC) was calculated4. EC maps were entered in
a multiple regression design (performed using SPM8), which included one
regressor of interest (tPSE) and three nuisance variables (gender,
education and age). Results were considered statistically significant at p<0.001
voxel level uncorrected with a threshold on the cluster size k=400. The
threshold chosen included two cluster level FWE corrections: p<0.1 (k=450)
and p<0.05 (k=600).Results
At
the group level we found tPSE(mean±sd)=(667±27)ms (Fig.1). Brain graphs
were obtained from 40,000 voxels of the grey matter. Positive correlation
between EC and tPSE was found in the primary somatosensory cortex
bilaterally (BA3, [-45,-22, 54] and [48, -19, 56], p<0.05 FWE), the premotor
cortex bilaterally (BA6, [-52, -1, 50] and [42, -15, 63], p<0.05 FWE) and
the right superior parietal lobule (BA7, [18, -57, 69], p<0.1 FWE) (Fig.2).Discussion
We characterised the Sense of Agency
(SoA) as a function of the delay between an action and its effect. We
found that the functional network sustaining agency processing
comprised the premotor and somatosensory cortices bilaterally, and the right
superior parietal lobule. Such prefronto-parietal circuitry is
densely connected and represents the pattern of a highly specialized network
dedicated to the conscious experience of controlling external events. This
findings support the comparator model account of SoA5, which
suggests intensive crosstalk between motor and somatosensory areas, calling for
a somatosensory-motor coding of sense of control6.Conclusion
Here
we demonstrated for the first time, that functional connectivity at rest in
healthy subjects is related to self-agency attribution when action feedback is
temporally distorted. Such findings are most informative for the debate
regarding the relative contribution of physiological/bottom-up and top-down
processes in self-agency attribution. They also provide a conceptual framework
for interpreting disorders of SoA in neuropsychiatric illnesses characterized
by both disrupted somatosensory systems and aberrant resting state activity.Acknowledgements
No acknowledgement found.References
1) Glover GH, Li TQ &
Ress D. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion
effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 162–7 (2000). 2) Chang C & Glover GH.
Effects of model-based physiological noise correction on default mode network anti-correlations
and correlations. Neuroimage 47, 1448–59 (2009). 3) Power et al.
Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state
fMRI. Neuroimage 84, 320-341 (2014). 4) Gili T. et al. The thalamus and brainstem act as key
hubs in alterations of human brain network connectivity induced by mild
propofol sedation. J. Neurosci. 33, 4024–31 (2013). 5) Blakemore SJ, Wolpert DM & Frith CD. Abnormalities in the
awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 6, 237–242
(2002). 6) Cui F et al. Functional
magnetic resonance imaging connectivity analyses reveal efference-copy to
primary somatosensory area, BA2. PLoS One 9, (2014).