Junyu Guo1, John O. Glass1, JungWon Hyun1, Yimei Li1, Heather Conklin1, Lisa Jacola1, Ching-Hon Pui1, Sima Jeha1, and Wilbrun E. Reddick1
1St Jude Children's Research Hospital, Memphis, TN, United States
Synopsis
Acute lymphoblastic
leukemia survivors may have significant deficits in processing speed and
working memory even when treated with only chemotherapy. We investigate the
relationship of diffusion tensor imaging metrics in an a priori brain
structural network with neurocognitive functions such as processing speed and
working memory. We found that fractional anisotropy values in the structural
network were significantly positively associated with processing speed
performance in two MR exams two years apart, and axial diffusivity values were
negatively associated with working memory in the MR exam at the end of therapy.
These findings may provide potential evidence
for a structural neurocognitive network.
Purpose
Acute lymphoblastic
leukemia (ALL) is the most common form of cancer in childhood and adolescence. 5-year
survival rates have improved to above 90% with contemporary chemotherapy alone.
However, the chemotherapy drugs have also been associated with chronic
neurotoxicity, which can result in impaired neurocognitive functions such as
worse working memory and slower processing speed. Previously, we built a structural
cognitive network from functional networks (central executive network, salience
network, and subcortical network) based on fMRI studies (1-7). This structural
cognitive network mainly includes dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC),
posterior parietal cortex (PPC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, and
subcortical cortex (basal ganglia and thalamus). To compensate for the low
angular and spatial resolution of the clinically acquired diffusion tensor
imaging (DTI) data, we created structural connectivity pathways using high
resolution DTI data from the Human Connectome Project (HCP). In a previous preliminary
study with a single MRI exam from 143 ALL survivors at end of chemotherapy, we found
that DTI metrics in most pathways were significantly associated with working
memory and processing speed performance. Here, we expanded our study to include
200 ALL survivors each with two MRI exams to further explore the association of
DTI metrics with neurocognitive performance. Methods
Two MRI exams were acquired two years apart early in therapy at re-induction
I and the end of therapy in 200 ALL survivors (121 male, 79 female, age at MR1
(re-induction) 7.2±4.4 , age
at MR2 (end of therapy)/ Cognitive Assessment 9.4±4.4 years).
All subjects were treated on a chemotherapy-only protocol, Total Therapy Study
XVI (NCT00549848). Low resolution DTI data was acquired with 12 directions and
a spatial resolution of 1.8 ×1.8 ×3.0 mm on a Siemens 3T scanner due to the
time limitation and were processed with SPM8 (fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/).
We evaluated all subjects using a pre-built cognitive network
(Fig. 1) using high-resolution DTI data from the Human Connectome Project (7). DTI metrics such as mean values of fractional
anisotropy (FA), axial diffusivity (AD), and radial diffusivity (RD) were
quantified in each pathway for 200 ALL survivors and 81 adult healthy
volunteers from the HCP. Processing speed was assessed using the WJ-III
Decision Speed test while working memory was assessed using the WJ Auditory
Working Memory test and the Digit Span Backward (DSB) test.
A multiple linear regression model was
used to fit the data. The model can be expressed as
$$Y_i=\beta_0+\beta_1DTI_i+\beta_2Age_i+\varepsilon_i$$
where $$$Y_i$$$ is a neurocognitive measure for the i-th
subject. We included age as a covariate to adjust for the age effect on the
neurocognitive measures as well as DTI measures. We applied the false discovery
rate (FDR) correction procedure of Benjamini and Hochberg to correct the
p-values for multiple testing. Results were considered significant at the P
< 0.05 level.
Results
The mean FA values in
both MR1 and MR2 were significantly associated with WJ-III Decision Speed in
most of pathways bilaterally (Table1).
The mean RD was significantly associated with Decision Speed only at
MR2, but not at MR1. The mean AD was significantly associated with WJ Auditory Working
Memory scores in most of pathways at both MR2 and ΔMR (Table
2). Consistently, the mean AD values in most of pathways at MR2 were
significantly associated with DSB measures, which also assess working memory
(Table 3). In addition, the mean RD at
MR2 shows significant association with DSB measures. Neither mean AD nor RD
showed any significant association with DSB measures at MR1 and ΔMR (Table
3). None of these DTI metrics had any
significant association with the processing speed and working memory measures
in adult volunteers.Discussion / Conclusion
Both larger FA and smaller
RD may indicate a higher degree of myelination in white matter. The thicker myelin sheaths enable signals to
transmit faster in axons. These facts are consistent with our findings: a
positive association of FA and a negative association of RD with processing
speed. The association between FA and processing speed is so strong that it is
evident at MR1, which was two years earlier than the cognitive assessment. In
contrast, AD and RD were negatively associated with working memory at MR2. Increases
in AD and RD may indicate declines in working memory ability due to brain
injury as a result of CNS-directed chemotherapy. Our findings were consistent
with the previous results from a smaller cohort of subjects, and provide
potential evidence for a structural neurocognitive network, which could be
further used for evaluating survivors at greatest risk for reduced
neurocognitive outcomes.Acknowledgements
This work was supported by Cancer Center Support Grant P30 CA-21765 from the National Cancer Institute at the National institutes of Health,Bethesda, MD, and by the American Lebanese Syrian Associated Charities (ALSAC), Memphis, TN. References
1.
Bressler
S, Memon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods
and principles. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (2010), 277-290.
2.
Memon V. Developmental pathways to functional
brain networks: emerging principles. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 (2013), 627-640.
3.
Seeley, W. et al. Dissociable intrinsic
connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J.
Neuosci. 27 (2007), 2349-2356.
4.
Frank
M. et al. Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: a computational model.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 1 (2001), 137-160.
5.
Collette F. et al. Exploration of the neural
substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging. Neuroscience.
139 (2006), 209-221.
6.
O’Reilly R. et al. Making working memory work:
a computational model of learning in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia.
Neural Comput. 18 (2006), 283-328.
7.
Guo J. et al. DTI Association with Working
Memory and Speed in Cognitive Network Pathways. Proc of ISMRM, 24th, p3443,
Singapore, 2016.