Sohae Chung1,2, Els Fieremans1,2, Joseph F. Rath3, and Yvonne W. Lui1,2
1Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R), Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States, 2Bernard and Irene Schwartz Center for Biomedical Imaging, Department of Radiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States, 3Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States
Synopsis
The relationship between performance on working memory tasks
of increasing difficulty and white matter (WM) microstructure assessed by
diffusion kurtosis imaging (DKI) is investigated in a healthy adult population.
We demonstrate that higher mean kurtosis (MK) and radial kurtosis (RK)
correlate with performance on working memory tasks, particularly in frontal WM,
an area responsible for executive function, suggesting better working memory
performance with higher tissue complexity in frontal WM. Improving our understanding
of these associations will help determine the biological underpinning of
pathologies affecting cognition, as well as potentially informing and
monitoring interventions such as cognitive rehabilitation.
PURPOSE
Working
memory is a hierarchical system at the core of cognition, involving short-term
memory representational components as well as a general executive attention component. Impaired working memory is
associated with a range of neurological and psychiatric disorders, as well as
normal aging.1,2 Little is known about how working memory relates to
underlying brain microstructure. In this study, we investigate the relationship
between performance on working memory tasks of increasing difficulty and white
matter (WM) microstructure assessed by diffusion kurtosis imaging (DKI) in a
healthy adult population. Understanding these associations will shed light on
the biological underpinnings of memory function and pathology.3METHODS
We
studied 18 healthy individuals (34±9, 19-50 years old; 10 male). Subjects
underwent the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Forth Edition (WAIS-IV) subtests:
Digit Span Forward (DSF), Backward (DSB), Sequencing (DSS), and Letter-Number
Sequencing (LNS), in order of increasing complexity. Test scores were converted
to z-scores based on age, with higher scores indicating higher ability. MR
imaging was performed on a 3T MR scanner (Skyra, Siemens). DKI acquisition was
performed with 5 b-values (0.25,1,1.5,2,2,2.5ms/mm2) along with 6,20,20,30,60 diffusion
encoding directions and three images with b=0 using multiband (factor of 2)
echo-planar imaging for accelerated acquisitions. One b=0 image with reversed
phase encoding direction was also acquired for geometric distortion correction.
Other imaging parameters were: acquisition matrix = 88×88, image resolution =
2.5×2.5×2.5mm3, number of slices = 56, TR/TE = 4.9s/95ms, BW/pixel =
2104Hz, FOV = 220×220mm2, a GRAPPA factor of 2. Diffusion and
kurtosis parametric maps of mean, axial and radial diffusion coefficients (MD,
AD, RD), fractional anisotropy (FA), and mean, axial and radial kurtosis (MK,
AK, RK) were calculated. Tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS)4 was
performed with age and gender as covariates to test for significant
correlations between imaging metrics and performance of working memory tasks.
The resulting statistical maps from TBSS were thresholded at p<0.05 with
family-wise error (FWE) correction. Partial correlation coefficient was
calculated for significant voxels (p<0.05, FWE-corrected) on skeleton from
TBSS, adjusted for age and gender.RESULTS
Figure
1 and 2 show the spatial distribution of the TBSS analysis for MK and RK,
respectively, that were significantly correlated with performance on the LNS (p
< 0.05, FWE-corrected), but no correlations were found with other subtests. Other
metrics did not show areas of significant correlation
surviving FWE correction. In Figs. 1(d) and 2(d), we observed positive
correlation particularly in frontal WM regions including the genu of the corpus
callosum (gCC). Partial correlation coefficients for significant voxels on
skeleton from TBSS were 0.84 (MK vs LNS; p < 0.001) and 0.81 (RK vs LNS; p
< 0.001).DISCUSSION
DKI
has been recently proposed to characterize non-Gaussian property of water
diffusion, thereby it better reflects the complexity of the tissue
microenvionment of the brain, compared to DTI only. We demonstrate that higher
MK and RK correlate with performance on working memory tasks of increasing
difficulty, particularly in frontal WM which is responsible for executive
function, suggesting better working memory performance with higher tissue
complexity in frontal WM. Establishing data regarding structural association
with working memory is critical to understanding what happens in the aging
brain and in pathologic conditions.CONCLUSION
This
study shows that there are structural associations with working memory.
Elucidating the links between WM microstructure and working memory will have
generalizable value in a variety of normal and pathologic conditions including
aging, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and dementia.Acknowledgements
Supported in part by R01 NS039135-11, R21 NS090349 and P41 EB017183.References
1. Wingfield
A, et al. Does the capacity of working memory change with age. Exp Aging Res. 1988;14(2):103-107.
2.
Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J
Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6(4):348-357.
3. Cicerone
KD, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for
clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2000;81(12):1596-1615.
4. Smith SM
et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion
data. Neuroimage. 2006;31(4):1487-1505.