Maryam Falahpour1, Alican Nalci1, Chi Wah Wong1, and Thomas Liu1
1Center for functional MRI, University of California San Diego, San Diego, CA, United States
Synopsis
Global signal regression (GSR) is a commonly used preprocessing
approach in the analysis of resting state fMRI data. Utilizing simultaneously
acquired EEG/fMRI data in humans, we found that GSR alters the correlation
between the resting-state BOLD fluctuations and EEG vigilance.
We show that GSR reveals BOLD-EEG correlations that are otherwise
obscured and use a time segmentation approach to argue that the observed
effects are not simply an artifact of GSR.
Purpose
Global signal regression (GSR) is a commonly used albeit
controversial preprocessing approach in the analysis of resting state fMRI
data.1 However, there is also growing evidence for the existence of
a significant neural component in the global signal2, suggesting
that GSR could potentially remove valuable information. Utilizing simultaneously
acquired EEG/fMRI data in humans, we examined the effect of GSR on the correlation
between resting-state BOLD fluctuations and EEG vigilance measures. We show that GSR reveals correlations that are otherwise obscured and use a time
segmentation approach to argue that the observed effects are not simply an
artifact of GSR. Methods
EEG-fMRI data were simultaneously acquired
on 9 healthy subjects during three eyes-closed resting-state sessions using a 3T
GE MR750 system and a 64 channel EEG system (Brain Products). BOLD fMRI data were
acquired with echo planar imaging with 166 volumes, 3.4×3.4×5mm3
voxel size, 64×64 matrix size, TR=1.8s, TE=30ms. Preprocessing steps are described
in.2 Vigilance was defined as the average alpha amplitude (7-13Hz)
divided by the average amplitude in the delta and theta (1-7Hz) bands,2
and the resulting time course was convolved with a hemodynamic response
function. The global signal (GS) was formed by averaging the percent change BOLD
time series across all brain voxels. For each run, the temporal correlation
between EEG vigilance and BOLD time courses was calculated before and after
GSR. Additionally, we split the time points into two subsets. The first set
contained only time points where the GS magnitude was either high (above the
third quartile (Q3)) or low (below the first quartile (Q1)).
The second set contained points in the interquartile range (IQR: middle 50%).
Correlations between the vigilance and BOLD time courses were computed on each
set separately (without GSR). For each processing approach, a one sample t-test
(3dttest++ from AFNI with t-test randomization to minimize the FPR) was performed
on the Fisher-z transformed correlation maps. Results
Figure 1 shows group result z-score maps (from 3dttest++) for each
condition. Consistent with prior studies,3-4 we found significant
positive BOLD-vigilance correlations in the thalamus and negative correlations in
widespread regions of the brain, including the posterior cingulate, cuneus, precuneus,
lingual gyrus, and insula. After GSR, there was a significant reduction in the
magnitude and the extent of the negative correlations, whereas the extent of
the positive correlations in sub-regions of the cingulate gyrus increased. See
Figures 1A and 1B.
We examined the individual correlations (as z-values) averaged
within three ROIs showing significant BOLD-vigilance correlations. Figure 2A
shows that there was a significant increase in the z-values (indicative of an
increase in correlation between EEG and BOLD) with GSR (as compared to no GSR) in
ROI1 (anterior and middle cingulate gyrus) and ROI3 (lingual gyrus, calcarine
gyrus, cuneus) but no significant difference in ROI2 (thalamus).
Figure 3 displays the GS time series from a representative
subject. The high/low value points are marked with red crosses, while the
points in the interquartile range are marked with black circles. The spatial
pattern of significant BOLD-vigilance correlations obtained when using the high/low
subset (Figure 1C) is similar to that found for the original data (all time
points) in Figure 1A. The pattern of
correlations obtained when using the interquartile range (Figure 1D) is
similar to that found after GSR (Figure 1B).
In addition, the relationship between the z-values for the correlations
from the interquartile and high/low subsets (Figure 2B) is similar to that
found when comparing the correlations obtained with GSR and no GSR (Figure 2A). Discussion
We have shown that GSR alters the correlation between BOLD fluctuations
and EEG vigilance. The correlations obtained with GSR are similar to those
obtained when using time points in the interquartile range, suggesting that GSR
may help to reveal correlations in time segments where the GS magnitude is low.
It is important to note that the positive correlations observed in the anterior
and middle cingulate gyrus (Fig. 1B and 1D) are not readily observed in the
original data (Fig 1A). The similarity of the results obtained with the
original data and the high/low subset suggests that the original correlations
are dominated by data from the high/low subset making it difficult to observe the
BOLD-EEG coupling in the interquartile subset. Furthermore, because the
correlations in the cingulate gyrus can be observed in data from the
inter-quartile subset (without GSR), it is unlikely that they are simply an
artifact of GSR. Our findings suggest that GSR and time-segmentation approaches
may be useful for uncovering relations between BOLD and vigilance that might not
be detectable with conventional approaches.
Acknowledgements
No acknowledgement found.References
[1] Murphy et. al.,
Neuroimage 2009, 44:893-905.
[2] Wong et. al., Neuroimage 2013, 83:983-990.
[3] Goldman et. al.. Neuroreport 2002, 13: 2487-2492
[4] Liu et. al., Neuroimage 2012, 63:1060-1069.