Laurentius Huber 1, Daniel A Handwerker1, Andrew Hall1, David C Jangraw2, Javier Gonzalez-Castillo1, Maria Guidi3, Dimo Ivanov4, Benedikt A Poser4, and Peter A Bandettini1
1SFIM, NIMH, Bethesda, MD, United States, 2NIMH, United States, 3Max Planck Institute for human cognitive and Brain science, Leipzig, Germany, 4MBIC, Maastricht University, Netherlands
Synopsis
Measurements of depth-dependent
cortical activity provide insights on directional activity between brain areas.
While previous studies demonstrated the feasibility of human depth-dependent
fMRI, the stability and reliability of depth-dependent results are less
studied. In this work, we investigate sources of inconsistencies in
depth-dependent activity profiles. We find that depth-dependent activity
profiles are highly reproducible across different scanning sessions. They are,
however, quite variable within cortical areas across different cross sections
along the cortical ribbon. Only when depth-dependent profiles are considered with
respect to their location along the cortical ribbon, task-driven modulations of
input-output activity become consistent across participants.
Purpose
Measurements of layer-dependent cortical activity
provide insight on how feedforward/feedback functional connectivity affects a
given cortical area. A few promising studies looking at differently modulated
feedback activity [1,2,3], show that depth-dependent fMRI can capture
depth-dependent activity modulations. However, individual studies come to contradictory
conclusions (feedback in supragranular vs. infragranular layers) [1,2].
In this study, we sought to investigate the reproducibility,
consistency, and heterogeneity of cortical depth-dependent fMRI results. We
focus on differences of depth-dependent activity A) across different
sensory-motor tasks, B) across scan sessions within participants on different
days; C) across different cross sections within the cortical ribbon that we
call ‘cortical distances’; D) across different participants with different anatomical
features and folding patterns.Methods
Experiments in 14 volunteers were performed on a 7T
Siemens scanner with a 32-channel NOVA Medical head coil. Data acquisition procedures
were used as previously presented in [4]. In short: We used a multi-contrast
VASO-BOLD sequence with slices positioned to be approximately perpendicular to
the M1 cortex. (Nominal resolution = 0.75×0.75×1.5 mm3, TI1/TI2/TR = 1.1/2.6/3.0 s, 12 slices
with FLASH GRAPPA-2 [5], matrix 44x132, 3D-EPI readout [6]. Five 12-min
functional scans were collected per participant: (1) right-hand tapping with touch,
(2) left-hand tapping with touch, (3) right-hand tapping without touch, (4) no tapping
while being touched with an abrasive cushion, (5) resting state. Segmentation and cortical layering algorithms
(based on the equi-volume approach [7]) were applied directly on the EPI images
from the functional scans.
For cross-participant comparison and
cross-participant averaging, we developed a sub-sampling method across cortical
depths and cortical distances: depths are normalized based on GM/CSF and GM/WM borders.
Cortical distances are normalized based on the bending radius (red arrows in
Fig. 2A) and hand knob templates described in [8]. Task-specific responses are
then averaged in this 2D-grid.
Results
The method provides sufficient sensitivity and
specificity to identify depth-dependent activity features (Fig. 1) as shown
earlier [4]. Depth-dependent activity features, like a double-layer response,
can be reproduced across multiple scan sessions up to 6 months apart.
Activity maps acquired across
different days look very similar; two distinct active depths can be identified across
all scanning sessions (black arrows in Fig. 1). Fig. 2A/B depicts how a
standardized 2D grid that is seeded in the hand knob can be used to average the
depth-dependent fMRI responses for multiple tasks across cortical distances
(Fig. 2C) and cortical depths (Fig. 2D). These averaged 2D grids are
transformed back into the EPI space of one representative participant in Fig. 3
to investigate the heterogeneous fMRI responses across cortical distances. Cortical
distances within M1 have different responses (for instance, white and black
arrows in Fig. 3) [9]. For the response heterogeneity across tasks, average depth-dependent
profiles are replotted on top of each other in Fig. 4A. Using a depth-dependent
model assuming superposition of input and output across M1 layers [10] (Fig.
4B), cross-participant results can be investigated for task-specific
heterogeneity in a scatter plot (Fig. 4D).Discussion
The result that CBV-fMRI
seems to be more distinct across cortical depths compared to BOLD-fMRI (Figs.
1/2D/4A) is consistent with previous single-participant analysis [4]. The
reproducibility of depth-specific activity patterns across scans (Fig. 1)
provides confidence that the patterns are indicative of layer-dependent, neurally
driven activity and not only session-specific artifacts. Accounting for
cross-participant variability of anatomical folding patterns, consistent
depth-dependent profiles could be obtained for 4 different stimuli with
different input-output characteristics. These depth-dependent results are
consistent with the animal literature: the modulation of sensory input (e.g., tapping
with vs. without touch) modulates the activity in upper layers. Modulation of
the output (e.g., tapping with touch vs. touch only) modulates deeper layers.Conclusion
Depth-dependent
results are reproducible across days. Depth-dependent results can be compared across
participants with a new approach using a standardized 2D grid. Depth-dependent profiles
are highly variable within cortical areas. This suggests that depth-dependent
fMRI will be a useful tool to investigate directional activity in
neuroscientific application.Acknowledgements
We thank Kâmil Uludag for the suggestion to introduce the more precise
descriptive terminology of cortical ‘depths’ and ‘distances’ instead of
‘layers’ and ‘columns’. Initial single-participant results of this study have been
presented at last year’s ISMRM in Singapore #948. This research is supported by
the American NIMH Intramural Research
Program.
References
[1] Muckli et al., Current
Biology, 2015, 20:232690-2695;
[2] Kok et al., Current Biology,
2016, 26: 371-376;
[3] Huber et al., NeuroImage, 2015,
107:23-33;
[4] Huber et al., ISMRM, 2016, #948;
[5] Talagala
et al., MRM, 2015, 75:2362-2371;
[6] Poser et
al., NeuroImage, 2010, 51:261-266;
[7] Waehnert
et al., NeuroImage, 2014, 93:210-220;
[8] Caulo et
al., Americ. J. Neuroradiology, 2007, 28:1480-1485;
[9] Havlicek et al., OHBM, 2016,
#7273
[10] Weiler et
al., Nature Neuroscience, 2008, 11:360-366;