Disadvantage of social sensitivity: Interaction of oxytocin receptor genotype (OXTR rs53576) and childhood maltreatment on limbic gray matter
Harald Kugel1, Udo Dannlowski2,3, Dominik Grotegerd2, Ronny Redlich2, Nils Opel 2, Katharina Dohm2, Dario Zaremba2, Anne Groegler2, Juliane Schwieren2, Thomas Suslow4, Patricia Ohrmann 2, Jochen Bauer1,2, Axel Krug3, Tilo Kircher3, Christa Hohoff2, Katharina Domschke5, Andreas Jansen3, Pienie Zwitserlood6, Markus Heinrichs7,8, Volker Arolt2, Walter Heindel1, and Bernhard T. Baune9

1Department of Clinical Radiology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany, 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany, 3Department of Psychiatry, University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany, 4Department of Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany, 5Department of Psychiatry, University of Wuerzburg, Wuerzburg, Germany, 6Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany, 7Department of Psychology, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany, 8Freiburg Brain Imaging Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany, 9School of Medicine, Discipline of Psychiatry, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia

Synopsis

Oxytocin is a pro-social and anxiolytic neuropeptide, especially if the G-allele of a common polymorphism (rs53576) in the oxytocin receptor gene is present. Recent studies suggest, however, a detrimental role of this allele in the context of childhood maltreatment. Structural MRI data show reduced gray matter volumes of the ventral striatum, fMRI shows increased amygdala responsiveness associated with increased CTQ (maltreatment) score. Thus for individuals with adverse childhood experiences the G-allele may be a vulnerability factor.

Purpose

Oxytocin has received much attention as a pro-social and anxiolytic neuropeptide (1). Its role in trust, social attachment, anxiety, fear conditioning, and amygdala responsiveness to emotional stimuli has been shown (1,2). In human studies, the G-allele of a common variant (rs53576) in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) has been associated with protective properties such as reduced stress response, and higher receptiveness for social support (3-5). In contrast, recent studies suggest a detrimental role of the rs53576 G-allele in the context of childhood maltreatment (6,7). To further elucidate the role of OXTR, gene x maltreatment (GxE) interaction on brain structure and function was investigated, based on the assumptions that (A) the previously described effect of reduced gray matter volumes in limbic areas after childhood maltreatment (8,9) is stronger in GG homozygotes than in A-allele carriers, and (B) maltreatment related amygdala responsiveness to emotional facial expressions (9-12) is stronger in G-allele than in A-allele carriers.

Methods

N=309 healthy adults underwent structural and functional MRI during a frequently used emotional face-matching task. All subjects were genotyped for OXTR rs53576. Childhood maltreatment was assessed using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (13). Reward dependence (RD), a pro-social trait, was assessed using Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) scores (14). Gray matter volumes were investigated by means of voxel-based morphometry (VBM) across the entire brain. MR images were acquired on a 3 T-MR scanner (Gyroscan Intera 3T, Philips, Best, NL) with a 3D fast gradient echo sequence (TFE), TR/TE/FA 7.4 ms/3.4 ms/9°, inversion prepulse every 815 ms, acquired over a FoV of 256 mm (FH, frequency encoding) x 204 mm (AP, phase encoding) x 160 mm (RL, phase encoding, nominal slice selection direction) with cubic voxels of 1 mm edge length, reconstructed to 0.5 mm. The VBM8-toolbox was used for preprocessing the structural images with default parameters within a unified model (15) including DARTEL normalization. Group statistics were calculated using SPM8, CTQ scores were entered as covariate. In addition, to accomodate the skewed distribution of CTQ scores, a categorical model (history of maltreatment yes/no) was used. Amygdala responses to facial expressions were investigated using a paradigm consisting of 4 blocks of a face processing task - trios with faces of the Ekman & Friesen stimulus set (16) must be checked for matching faces - alternating with 5 blocks of a sensorimotor control task, frequently used for imaging genetics (9,17,18). fMRI data were acquired with a single shot EPI sequence, TR/TE/FA 2.1 s/30 ms/90°, 34 slices tilted 25° from the AC/PC line, no gap, matrix 64x64, cubic voxels with 3.6 mm edge length. After exclusions for technical reasons, N=264 datasets were available for fMRI analysis of bilateral amygdala, using a full-factorial model with genotype as between subjects factor and CTQ scores as covariate.

Results

While no significant main effects of genotype were detected, structural MRI data revealed a robust interaction of rs53576 genotype and CTQ-scores mapping specifically to the bilateral ventral striatum. GG homozygotes but not A-allele carriers showed strong gray matter reduction with increasing CTQ-scores (Fig. 1). The categorial model (maltreatment y/n) yielded almost identical results. An effect of gender on the GxE (gene x environment) effect was not observed. Concerning fMRI, a main effect of maltreatment mapping to the right amygdala was found, as well as a main effect of genotype: GG homozygotes showed increased amygdala response to angry and fearful faces, compared to A-allele carriers (Fig. 2). No GxE interaction was found. In turn, RD scores from the TPQ showed that lower ventral striatum gray matter volumes were associated with lower reward dependence.

Discussion

In GG homozygotes, a negative correlation of gray matter volume in the ventral striatum and the degree of childhood maltreatment was detected, while A-allele carriers did not show this detrimental maltreatment effect. Second, amygdala responsiveness to emotional cues is stronger in GG homozygotes than in A-allele carriers.The higher receptiveness for social cues in GG homozygotes may result in a stronger profit from beneficial environments, but less resilience to detrimental effects of childhood maltreatment, while A-allele carriers are less affected by positive or negative aspects of their early environment.

In conclusion, the findings suggest that the G-allele constitutes a vulnerability factor for specifically altered limbic brain structure in individuals with adverse childhood experiences, complemented by increased limbic responsiveness to emotional interpersonal stimuli. While oxytocinergic signaling facilitates attachment and bonding in supportive social environments, this attunement for social cues may turn disadvantageous under early adverse conditions.

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by grants from the German Research Foundation (DFG; grant FOR 2107; DA1151/5-1) and Innovative Medizinische Forschung (IMF) of the Medical Faculty, University of Muenster (DA120903/DA111107/DA211021 to UD). We thank Ahmad Hariri for providing the fMRI paradigm.

References

(1) Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M, Nat Rev Neurosci 2011; 2: 524–538. (2) Heinrichs M, von Dawans B, Domes G, Front Neuroendocrinol 2009; 30: 548–557. (3) Kumsta R, Heinrichs M, Curr Opin Neurobiol 2013; 23: 11–16. (4) Rodrigues SM, Saslow LR, Garcia N, John OP, Keltner D, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106: 21437–21441. (5) Tost H, Kolachana B, Hakimi S, et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010; 107: 13936–13941. (6) McQuaid RJ, McInnis OA, Stead JD, Matheson K, Anisman H, Front Neurosci 2013; 7: 128. (7) Hostinar CE, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Dev Psychopathol 2014; 26: 465–477. (8) Edmiston EE, Wang F, Mazure CM, Guiney J, Sinha R, Mayes LC, Blumberg HP, Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2011; 165: 1069–1077. (9) Dannlowski U, Stuhrmann A, Beutelmann V, et al. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 71: 286–293. (10) Dannlowski U, Kugel H, Huber F, et al. Hum Brain Mapp 2013; 34: 2899–2909. (11) Van Harmelen A-L, van Tol M-J, Demenescu LR, et al. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 8: 362-369. (12) McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Kelly PA, et al. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202: 269–276. (13) Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, et al. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 1132–1136. (14) Weyers P, Krebs H, Janke W, Pers Individ Dif 1995; 19: 853–861. (15) Ashburner J, Friston KJ, Neuroimage 2005; 26: 839–851. (16) Ekman P, Friesen W V. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press 1976. (17) Dannlowski U, Kugel H, Franke F, et al. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011; 36: 1897-1885. (18) Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, et al. Science 2002; 297: 400–403.

Figures

Fig. 1: Interaction of OXTR rs53576 and childhood maltreatment: Effect of CTQ-scores on gray matter volume dependent on OXTR rs53576 genotype (N=170 A-carriers vs. N=139 GG-homozygotes). Left: Display of Interaction (color bar, Z-value). Voxel-threshold p<0.001, cluster-threshold k=458. Right, Top: CTQ-scores, dimensional measure. Bottom: Dichotomized groups (maltreatment: yes/no). Error bars, s.e.m.

Fig. 2: Main effect of OXTR rs53576 genotype on amygdala responsiveness to angry/fearful faces. Left: Genotype dependent amygdala responsiveness (N=144 A-carriers vs. N=120 GG-homozygotes), p<0.05, k=38 voxels (no left amygdala cluster at this threshold). Color bar, t-value. Right: fMRI contrast at x=30/y=0/z=-20, dependent on genotype. Error bars, s.e.m



Proc. Intl. Soc. Mag. Reson. Med. 24 (2016)
4150