Helena Melero1, Susana Borromeo1, Alexandra Cristobal-Huerta1, Eva Manzanedo1, and Juan Antonio Hernandez-Tamames1
1Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Madrid, Spain
Synopsis
Neuroimaging experiments on grapheme-color
synesthesia have provided evidence of structural and functional peculiarities
in the synesthetic brain and several explanatory models have been proposed.
Nevertheless, data from other modalities are needed in order to test their
predictions. For the first time, we investigated brain activity in response to
olfactory stimuli in multiple synesthetes. Results showed differential activity
in areas that participate in high level visual processing, memory, language,
lexical meaning and emotion. These findings suggest that the Conceptual
Mediation Model and the Emotional Binding Theory may be complementary and
reinforce the idea that meaning and emotion are intrinsically related
processes. Introduction
Magnetic resonance imaging experiments on
synesthesia have provided empirical evidence of structural and functional
peculiarities in the synesthetic brain. The majority of this research was
conducted on grapheme-color synesthesia (GCS), given that it is one of the most
prevalent modalities and due to the fact that visual features are more
accessible for scientific studies; on the basis of this knowledge, different
explanatory models of synesthesia have been proposed. For example, the Cascaded
Cross-Tuning model (CCT) suggests that the neural basis of synesthesia is
localized in regions that participate in the processing of the inducer (e.g. a
letter) and the concurrent (e.g. a color). The model predicts that these regions
are likely adjacent and show increased activation, anatomical variations and/or
rapid coactivation in synesthetes (1). Evidence from GCS supports this model,
whereas data available from other modalities are not enough to test its
predictions. Other models, such as the Conceptual Mediation Model (2) or the
Emotional Binding Theory (3,4) predict additional activity in areas that
participate in lexical meaning and/or emotion. In order to shed light on this
debate, data from other modalities of synesthesia are needed. Interestingly,
eleven modalities of synesthesia include odors as inducers or concurrents.
Emotions, lexemes, touch, personalities, vision or sound can trigger olfactory
synesthesias. Additionally, aromas can trigger flavors, temperature, sounds,
touch and visual experiences such as color. So far, olfactory synesthesias have
been studied only from phenomenological (5) and behavioral perspectives (6).
Thus, in order to provide new empirical evidence about the neural basis of
synesthesia, which can help test current explanatory models of the phenomenon,
we conducted the first fMRI experiment on olfaction in a group of multiple
synesthetes.
Methods
Twelve participants (6 synesthetes: 3 men and 3
women, mean age 36 ± 22.18; 6 non-synesthetes: 3 men and 3 women, mean age 54 ±
19.28) were presented with two mixed olfactory-trigeminal odors: mint and an
aqueous dilution of 1-butanol (4% concentration) through an fMRI compatible
olfactometer (7). Each odor was delivered 9 times in two different series and
recorded in a single session. An event-related design (stimulus duration: 2 s;
stimulus onset asynchrony: 22 s) was used to minimize the effects of
habituation. The fMRI data were acquired in a 3.0 T Signa HDxt MR scanner (GE
Healthcare, Waukesha, WI, USA) with an eight-channel head coil (GE Coils,
Cleveland, OH). Functional images were obtained using a T2* weighted
echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (echo time = 25 ms; flip angle = 77°; matrix
size = 64 × 64; field of view = 22 cm; repetition time = 2 s; number of slices
= 23). Twenty-three contiguous ACPC-oriented slices (3 mm thickness) covering
the inferior part of the brain were acquired. Fieldmap correction (8,9) was
applied in order to minimize the impact of susceptibility artifacts. SPM8
implemented in MATLAB R2014a (Mathworks, Inc.) was used for data analysis. A
two-sample t-test analysis (synesthetes vs. controls) was applied in order to
observe intergroup differences in olfaction.
Results
Several areas of the brain showed intergroup
differences (Figure 1: synesthetes > controls; the reverse contrast did not show any significant results). First, areas that participate in high
level visual processing, such as the left perirhinal cortex (BA 36) and the
right fusiform gyrus (BA 37). Second, several areas that belong to emotional
networks, such as the left temporal pole, bilateral Insula (BA 13), the right
middle frontal gyrus (BA 47), the left anterior/inferior temporal lobe and the
right medial frontal gyrus (BA 10). Third, regions that participate in language
processing, such as the right superior temporal gyrus (BA 21) and the left
superior temporal gyrus (BA 22). It is interesting to note that these regions
exhibit other complementary functions. For example, the left perirhinal cortex
participates in memory, the left anterior/inferior temporal lobe provides
meaning and the medial frontal gyrus is involved in working memory.
Conclusion
The neurofunctional basis of olfactory synesthesias
is distributed, including visual, lexical, and emotional areas. These data
support the idea that the synesthetic brain is able to activate semantic
networks in the absence of purely symbolic stimuli, in consonance with the
Conceptual Mediation Model. Additionally, these data reflect the overactivation
of emotional networks that may be acting as multisensory integrators between
the inducer and the concurrent, in line with the Emotional Binding Theory.
These findings suggest that both models may be complementary, and reinforce the
idea that meaning and emotion are intrinsically related processes.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported
by Universidad Rey Juan Carlos (Biomarkers Based on Magnetic Resonance Imaging
in Neurological Diseases: TEC2012-39095-C03-01).References
1. Hubbard EM, Brang D, Ramachandran VS.
The cross-activation theory at 10. J. Neuropsychol. 2011;5:152–177.
2. Chiou R, Rich AN. The role of conceptual
knowledge in understanding synaesthesia: Evaluating contemporary findings from
a “hub-and-spokes” perspective. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:105.
3. Melero H, Peña-Melián A, Ríos-Lago M,
Pajares G, Hernández-Tamames JA, Álvarez-Linera J. Grapheme-color synesthetes
show peculiarities in their emotional brain: cortical and subcortical evidence
from VBM analysis of 3D-T1 and DTI data. Exp. brain Res. 2013;227:343–53.
4. Melero H, Ríos-Lago M, Peña-Melián, A., Álvarez-Linera J. Achromatic synesthesias
- A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 2014;98:416–424.
5. Stevenson RJ, Tomiczek C.
Olfactory-induced synesthesias: a review and model. Psychol Bull 2007;133:294–309.
6. Russell A, Stevenson RJ, Rich AN.
Chocolate smells pink and stripy: Exploring olfactory-visual synesthesia. Cogn.
Neurosci. 2015:1–12.
7. Borromeo.S, Hernández-Tamames, J.A.,
Luna, G., Machado, F., Malpica, N., Toledano, A., 2010. Objective Assessment of
olfactory function using Functional Magnetic Resonance. IEEE Transactions on
Instrumentation & Mesurements 59 (10): 2601-2608
8. Jezzard P, Balaban RS. Correction for
geometric distortion in echo planar images from B0 field variations. Magn.
Reson. Med. 1995;34:65–73.
9. Schneider E, Glover G. Rapid in vivo
proton shimming. Magn. Reson. Med. 1991;18:335–347.