Synopsis
Although East Asia
harbors the largest number of aging adults in the world, there is limited data
on longitudinal changes in brain structure and its relationship with
domain-specific cognition. We tracked changes in brain volume and 5 cognitive functions
over 8 years among healthy older adults in the Singapore-Longitudinal Aging
Brain Study. After adjusting for intracranial volume, demographic factors, and
blood pressure, total cerebral atrophy was associated with faster decline in
verbal memory. Hippocampal atrophy and ventricular expansion were associated
with greater decline in verbal memory and executive functions. These findings
clarify the relationships between age-trends in neurobiology and cognition.Purpose
With increasing life expectancy in many societies, cognitive decline
accompanying aging or neurodegenerative disease has become a growing problem. Changes
in brain structures have been associated with cognitive decline in healthy
older adults (Henneman et al., 2009) and may precede the appearance of clinical
symptoms of dementia by several years (Carlson et al., 2008). Despite the
burgeoning elderly population in Asia, the bulk of published research on ageing
humans comes from the West. The present study seeks to provide an East Asian
perspective on longitudinal brain aging in a cohort of relatively healthy older
adults. Here, we analyzed structural brain MRI and neuropsychological data collected
over the course of 8 years to quantify annual changes in brain structures and functions
in 5 cognitive domains and to investigate the relationship between these brain
and cognition trends.
Methods
The current sample comprised 111 relatively healthy older adults (mean
age=67.1 years; SD=6.3 years) with no known active major medical conditions
other than treated, uncomplicated hypertension (33.3% at baseline) or diabetes
mellitus (9.9% at baseline).
Performance in 5
cognitive domains were assessed: speed of processing (Trial-Making Test-A, the
Symbol Search Task, and Symbol-Digit Modalities Test), executive function
(Categorical Verbal Fluency test, Design Fluency test, and Trial Making Test
B), attention (Digit Span Task and Spatial Span task), verbal memory (Rey
Auditory Verbal Learning Test), and visuo-spatial memory (Visual Paired
Associates test). Individual test scores from the subsequent 3 follow-up visits
were first z-transformed with
reference to baseline, and then converted to T-scores (group mean of T score at baseline = 50; SD = 10). For
each time point, a composite score was derived for each of the five cognitive
domains by averaging the T-scores of
the respective tasks.
High-resolution
images of the brain were acquired using a T1-weighted MEMPRAGE sequence with a
3T Siemens Tim Trio system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). There were 192
sagittal slices with the following scanning parameters: TR=2530ms, TI=1200ms, FA=7°,
FOV=256×256 mm, isotropic voxel dimensions of 1.0 mm. Automated measurements of
brain volumes were standardized across phases and performed using FreeSurfer
5.1.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). Here, we report findings regarding
total cerebral, gray and white matter, hippocampal, and ventricular volumes.
Estimated total intracranial volume (eTIV; Buckner et al., 2004) was used as a
covariate in all statistical analyses involving brain variables to compensate
for inter-individual differences in head size.
We used mixed-effects model to
quantify the annual change in brain volume and cognitive performance at both a
group and an individual levels. Partial correlations were performed to
investigate the relationships between longitudinal changes in cognitive
function and brain after controlling for eTIV, age, BMI, education, gender, and
blood pressure (systolic and diastolic) at baseline.
Results and Discussion
All
brain structures investigated showed significant annual volumetric decline (from
-0.53% to -0.94%/year), and the ventricles showed significant annual expansion
at 3.56%/year (Table 1). These annual changes were similar to those reported in
other longitudinal studies examining healthy aging (Jack et al., 2005). In
contrast, speed of processing was the only cognitive domain that revealed
significant decline associated with aging (mean=-0.92%/year), while performance
in the other four cognitive domains was relatively preserved with increasing
age (Table 1), corroborating the work of others documenting strong age effects
on speed of processing (Salthouse, 2009). Inter-individuals differences in
brain and cognitive changes over time were observed (Figures 1 and 2).
Associations
between longitudinal changes in brain structures and cognitive performance are
summarized in Table 2. After adjusting for eTIV, age, BMI, education, gender,
and blood pressure, greater total cerebral atrophy was significantly associated
with faster decline in verbal memory (partial r=0.24, p=0.02; Figure
3A), likely driven by the latter’s positive association with white matter
atrophy (partial r=0.20, p<0.05, Figure 3B). This association
is consistent with an early imaging study which linked white matter lesions
with poorer recall on word lists in older adults (Breteler et al., 1994), and
more recent work which revealed that greater disruption of white matter
pathways predicted MCI status (Delano-Wood et al., 2012).
Hippocampal atrophy and
ventricular expansion were associated with poorer verbal memory (partial r=0.21; p=0.03, Figure 3C, partial r=-0.22;
p=0.03, Figure 3D) and executive
function (partial r=0.22, p=0.03, Figure 3E, partial r=-0.23, p=0.02, Figure 3F). Similar associations have been documented for
western populations (Murphy et al., 2010;
Breteler et al., 1994). Using a cohort of relatively healthy Chinese older adults, we
have quantified the longitudinal changes in brain structures and cognitive functions,
and provided detailed characterization of the relationships between these brain
and cognitive changes over an 8-year period.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Biomedical Research Council, Singapore
(04/1/36/19/372) and the National Medical Research Council Singapore
(STaR/0004/2008).References
1.
Henneman WJ,
Sluimer JD, Barnes J, van der Flier WM, Sluimer IC, Fox NC, Scheltens P,
Vrenken H, Barkhof F. Hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer disease: added
value over whole brain volume measures. Neurology. 2009;72(11):999-1007.
2.
Carlson NE,
Moore MM, Dame A, Howieson D, Silbert LC, Quinn JF, Kaye JA. Trajectories of
brain loss in aging and the development of cognitive impairment. Neurology.
2008;70(11):828-33.
3.
Buckner RL,
Head D, Parker J, Fotenos AF, Marcus D, Morris JC, Snyder AZ. A unified approach
for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented
adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and
validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. NeuroImage.
2004;23(2):724-38.
4.
Jack CR, Jr.,
Shiung MM, Weigand SD, O'Brien PC, Gunter JL, Boeve BF, Knopman DS, Smith GE,
Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC. Brain atrophy rates predict subsequent
clinical conversion in normal elderly and amnestic MCI. Neurology.
2005;65(8):1227-31.
5.
Salthouse TA.
Decomposing age correlations on neuropsychological and cognitive variables.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS.
2009;15(5):650-61.
6.
Breteler MM,
van Amerongen NM, van Swieten JC, Claus JJ, Grobbee DE, van Gijn J, Hofman A,
van Harskamp F. Cognitive correlates of ventricular enlargement and cerebral
white matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging. The Rotterdam Study.
Stroke; a journal of cerebral circulation. 1994;25(6):1109-15.
7.
Delano-Wood
L, Stricker NH, Sorg SF, Nation DA, Jak AJ, Woods SP, Libon DJ, Delis DC, Frank
LR, Bondi MW. Posterior cingulum white matter disruption and its associations
with verbal memory and stroke risk in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of
Alzheimer's disease : JAD. 2012;29(3):589-603.
8.
Murphy
EA, Holland D, Donohue M, McEvoy LK, Hagler DJ, Jr., Dale AM, Brewer JB,
Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging I. Six-month atrophy in MTL structures is
associated with subsequent memory decline in elderly controls. NeuroImage.
2010;53(4):1310-7.