Synopsis
Major depressive disorder (MDD) patients exhibit higher
rumination levels; repetitive thinking and focus on negative states. Rumination
is known to be associated with brain cortical midline and limbic structures, yet,
the underlying brain network topological organization remains unclear. Implementing
a graph-theory analysis we tested whether whole brain network connectivity
hierarchies during fMRI resting state are associated with rumination. We found
a significant correlation between right caudal anterior cingulate (cACC) connectivity
strength and subjective rumination tendency. This result emphasize the cACC
impact during self-reflective processing, which might serve as biomarker for
clinical diagnosis.
Introduction
Ruminative brooding is
conceptualized as repetitive thinking and focus on one’s distress and negative
mood states with high self-critical nature.1 Patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) exhibit
increased levels of rumination2 which have been found to increase the risk of
depressive relapse in remitted patients.3 In neuroimaging, rumination has been associated with
aberrant activity of the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex
(ACC), insula, temporal pole, hippocampus, and amygdala.4-8 Yet, little is known about alterations of the topological
organization of whole-brain networks during self-referential processing in
relation to rumination. Resting state functional
MRI (fMRI) offers a good modality to examine brain network connectivity when
resting and not engaging in specific cognitive tasks, when self-referential
processing such as rumination can predominate.5 In order to examine the whole brain functional topological
organization we applied a graph theoretical approach. We hypothesized that the functional
brain network topological organization will distinguish between MDD patients
and healthy controls (HC) and will also associate with individual
differences in self-reported rumination
scores. Methods
Thirteen MDD patients and 21 HC underwent resting state
fMRI. Data were acquired on a Siemens Magnetom 7T MRI scanner (Erlangen,
Germany). Functional images were processed using the multi-echo independent
component analysis implemented in the AFNI meica.py toolbox.9 Each
subjects anatomical T1 brain image was segmented into the Desikan-Killiany
Atlas10 applied
in FreeSurfer 6.0. The segmentation resulted in 84 regions of interest each representing a node of the network (Figure 1A). To
define the network edges, we calculated the partial correlation coefficients
between the regional mean time-series of all pairwise regions (excluding the
effects of all other regions) (Figure 1C). To enable comparison across participants, we used a
sparsity threshold S, which retains S% of the top connections for each
participant (Figure 1D). Using
the Brain Connectivity Toolbox,11 we examined the most common weighted network property
of node strength which is the sum of weights of links connected to
the node (Figure 1E). We
examined the node strength across a range of thresholds (10% < S <
30% in steps of 1%).12 We then calculated
the area under the curve for each network, which provides a summarized measure
independent of single threshold selection12 (Figure 1F).
We conducted a between-group t-test for each region's strength degree. All
results were corrected for multiple comparisons (corrected for number of nodes),
using false discovery rate (FDR) correction (q<0.05).13 Next, we conducted
Pearson correlations to assess the association between the regions strength
degree and the subjective ruminative brooding scores as assessed by the
Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS).14 Partial correlation was used to control for age and
gender as covariates. We then corrected for multiple comparisons using FDR (q<0.05)
(corrected for number of nodes).Results
The results revealed that MDD patients exhibited
the same hierarchy of regions strength in the network as HC during rest (Figure
2A). However, across all participates, higher strength of the right caudal ACC (cACC)
during rest was associated with greater ruminative brooding score (r=0.85,
p<0.0003, q FDR<0.05) (Figure 2B). The cACC was also positively
correlated with the rumination brooding score only among the HC group (r=0.65,
p<0.007, uncorrected), and with a trend among the MDD group (r=0.51,
p=0.12). Further exploration of the cACC functional connectivity exhibited
that greater ruminative brooding tendency was associated with increased
connectivity with the left pericalcarine (r=0.51, p<0.004, uncorrected)
and right superior parietal (r=0.39, p<0.03, uncorrected)(Figure
3). However, none of these results survived the FDR correction. Discussion
Our results showed that the MDD patients exhibit
the same manner of network topology as HC during resting state. Nevertheless, we
showed that the higher the subjects’ cACC strength within the whole brain
network the higher their tendency to ruminate. These
results are in line with previous studies showing ACC hyperactivity during
self-referential processing in MDD patients.8,15,16 The overall
increased cACC strength in the brain network indicates higher correlations to
the rest of the regions, which might imply of its maladaptive functionality. Specific
increased connectivity with the pericalcarine and right superiorparietal may govern
this hyper-connectivity, however further research is needed to improve
statistics. Our findings add to existing research by inspecting a large-scale
brain network hierarchy and revealing the cACC as a critical node in context of
rumination.Conclusion
In conclusion, our results highlight the cACC
impact on the whole brain network during resting state among normal and MDD
patients in relation to ruminative brooding. The network hierarchy and
specifically the role of the cACC within the functional brain network in
context of self-referential processing might serve as biomarkers for clinical
diagnosis and neuromodulation-based interventions for MDD patients.Acknowledgements
NIH R01 MH109544Icahn
School of Medicine Capital Campaign
Translational and Molecular Imaging Institute
References
1 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J. & Fredrickson, B. L. Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of abnormal psychology 102, 20-28 (1993).
2 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E. & Lyubomirsky, S. Rethinking Rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3, 400-424, doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x (2008).
3 Roberts, J. E., Gilboa, E. & Gotlib, I. H. Ruminative Response Style and Vulnerability to Episodes of Dysphoria: Gender, Neuroticism, and Episode Duration. Cognitive Therapy and Research 22, 401-423, doi:10.1023/a:1018713313894 (1998).
4 Fossati, P. et al. In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. The American journal of psychiatry 160, 1938-1945, doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1938 (2003).
5 Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L. & Raichle, M. E. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98, 4259-4264, doi:10.1073/pnas.071043098 (2001).
6 Ochsner, K. N. & Gross, J. J. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in cognitive sciences 9, 242-249, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010 (2005).
7 van der Meer, L., Costafreda, S., Aleman, A. & David, A. S. Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 34, 935-946, doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.004 (2010).
8 Nejad, A., Fossati, P. & Lemogne, C. Self-Referential Processing, Rumination, and Cortical Midline Structures in Major Depression. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00666 (2013).
9 Kundu, P., Inati, S. J., Evans, J. W., Luh, W. M. & Bandettini, P. A. Differentiating BOLD and non-BOLD signals in fMRI time series using multi-echo EPI. NeuroImage 60, 1759-1770, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.028 (2012).
10 Desikan, R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31, 968-980, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 (2006).
11 Rubinov, M. & Sporns, O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage 52, 1059-1069, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 (2010).
12 Korgaonkar, M. S., Fornito, A., Williams, L. M. & Grieve, S. M. Abnormal Structural Networks Characterize Major Depressive Disorder: A Connectome Analysis. Biological Psychiatry 76, 567-574, doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.018 (2014).
13 Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological) 57, 289-300, doi:10.2307/2346101 (1995).
14 Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R. & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Rumination Reconsidered: A Psychometric Analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research 27, 247-259, doi:10.1023/a:1023910315561 (2003).
15 Kessler, H. et al. Individualized and Clinically Derived Stimuli Activate Limbic Structures in Depression: An fMRI Study. PLOS ONE 6, e15712, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0015712 (2011).
16 Cooney, R. E., Joormann, J., Eugène, F., Dennis, E. L. & Gotlib, I. H. Neural correlates of rumination in depression. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 10, 470-478, doi:10.3758/cabn.10.4.470 (2010).